Skip to ContentGo to accessibility pageKeyboard shortcuts menu
OpenStax Logo
Introduction to Behavioral Neuroscience

10.3 Our Brain Gets Involved – Responsibilities of Upper Motor Systems

Introduction to Behavioral Neuroscience10.3 Our Brain Gets Involved – Responsibilities of Upper Motor Systems

Learning Objectives

By the end of this section, you should be able to

  • 10.3.1 Compile a descriptive story regarding how the following UMN brain structures collaborate with distinct contributions to movement formulation and engagement: Prefrontal and premotor cortices; Cerebellum; Basal ganglia; and Primary Motor cortex (M1)
  • 10.3.2 Enumerate the regions of control within the homunculus of M1 anatomically along the precentral gyrus.
  • 10.3.3 Differentiate the complimentary contributions to background motor control from the basal ganglia and cerebellum in terms of muscle contribution selection and the smooth stopping and starting.
  • 10.3.4 Understand the source, related circuitry, and outward expressions of the following disease states: Cerebellar Ataxia; Huntington’s disease; and Parkinson’s disease.
  • 10.3.5 Describe how final movement selections are made in M1 according to the findings of Apostolos Georgopoulos and how that translates into a sort of democracy of movement trajectory determination.

In this section, we will explore the contributions of UMN systems to voluntary movements. Progressively, we will discuss ascending signals providing critical decision-making information to UMN areas, and descending implementation signals to the lower areas (brainstem and spinal cord). We’ll address three major cortical areas:

  1. The prefrontal cortices, the most anterior of movement-related cortices where major executive decisions are made.
  2. The collection of regions referred to as the premotor cortices (dorsal, ventral, cingulate, supplementary). These coordinate which muscles should be activated when new game plans are formulated, and receive feeds from posterior sensory cortices, the basal ganglia, and the cerebellum through the thalamic way station.
  3. Finally, we will discuss the primary motor cortex, or M1. We will find that this area continues to compile inputs and formulate final detailed movement decisions prior to sending commands to the LMNs in the brainstem and spinal cord.

Our lower motor systems clearly need guidance if we are ever going to move how we want. In this section, we will learn how the upper motor structures are poised to deliberate, compose, compile, and then direct the lower motor neurons.

The Prefrontal Cortex – Integrating our wants and needs with circumstances

Within the most anterior portions of the cerebral cortex behind our foreheads and over the orbits of our eyes, prefrontal cortices begin the earliest portions of conscious decision-making and are included as motor regions for this reason (see Figure 10.12). Willed movements depend on both decisions and integration of emotions and desires within internal representations concerning our circumstances in the world. We need to understand problems we face prior to acting and speaking in response to them, lest our actions be thoughtless or wildly inappropriate. For example, prefrontal lobotomy patients are unable to plan for their futures, and fail to self-monitor as actions occur (Milner & Petrides, 1984; Malloy et al., 1993; Chirchiglia et al., 2019). This clearly defines these cortical regions as the locus of deciding what to do. As indicated in Figure 10.12, this area receives necessary decision-making information from many other areas. After processing, it sends this information to multiple regions, providing both the feeling of the decision and the action chosen.

Prefrontal interactions Top: Diagram of a human brain surface with arrows indicating pathways to the prefrontal cortex. Bottom: Diagram of a human brain surface with arrows indicating pathways away from the prefrontal cortex.
Figure 10.12 Image credit: Image inspired by Fuster, J. 2015. The Prefrontal Cortex. 5th Ed. eBook ISBN: 9780124080607

Premotor cortices

Once an executive decision about a desired movement has been made in the PFC, the conscious compilation of movements into useful patterns occurs in the premotor cortices. These cortices reside in the lateral and dorsal regions of the frontal lobe, behind the prefrontal areas. This general region compiles movements by combining multiple muscle sets across space (different simultaneous contractions combining, adjusting movement trajectory) and time (different movements in sequence). See Figure 10.13 for an example of these functions, as gymnast Simone Biles plans a complex flip. Her premotor cortex will be essential for planning force and distance while her supplementary motor area stitches together the order of movements she will need.

Top: Left: Photo of gymnast Simone Biles's face/head. Thought bubble above her head indicate 'premotor cortex: planning for and distance' and 'supplementary motor area: planning order of operations.' Right: Picture of Simone Biles flipping in the air. text indicates 'Primary motor cortex: engage extensors and flexors to move'. Bottom: Left: Diagram of human brain surface with highlighting to show premotor area, supplementary motor area and primary motor cortex. Right: coronal view of major divisions of primary motor cortex, showing drawings of represented body parts lying along the surface of the cortex.
Figure 10.13 Premotor versus motor processing Image credit: Thinking Simone Biles: By Agência Brasil Fotografias - EUA levam ouro na ginástica artística feminina; Brasil fica em 8º lugar, CC BY 2.0, https://commons.wikimedia.org/w/index.php?curid=50584958; Flipping Simone Biles: By Agência Brasil Fotografias - EUA levam ouro na ginástica artística feminina; Brasil fica em 8º lugar, CC BY 2.0, https://commons.wikimedia.org/w/index.php?curid=50584654

Of course, Simone Biles has done these flips many times. Her repeated practice has made much of her motor planning subconscious. This process of honing motor movements into habitual, subconscious patterns relies strongly on interactions of premotor cortices with other cortical structures such as the striatum (basal ganglia), and cerebellum. These two areas become involved more heavily during acquisition (where movement skills are packaged), consolidation (where movement skills are honed into quickly accessible repertoire habits or motor programs), and retention (where established repertoire movements are stored so they can be accessed quickly in response to key circumstances). Both primates and rats suffer deficits in learning formal, visually directed motor tasks after the removal of premotor cortices – further evidence of this region's critical role in retaining learned skill and movement adaptation for future endeavors.

The premotor areas' primary output is regulation of M1 neurons, which connect extensively to LMNs. For many years, the premotor cortices were believed to influence motor movement exclusively via their regulation of the primary motor cortex. However, more recent evidence shows stimulation of the premotor cortices yielding combinations of motor responses in harmony across both sides of the body. Thus premotor cortex engages built-in and learned complex combinations of actions, within an organism's more sophisticated repertoire (Geyer et al., 2000). Premotor cortices send descending axons into the spinal cord, meaning that M1 is not the only direct regulator of LMNs. This is important to keep in mind.

The basal ganglia

This cluster of subcortical nuclei includes the main input regions, caudate and putamen nuclei, with circuit components extending into the external and internal globus pallidus, subthalamic nucleus, and substantia nigra reticulata. Importantly, in most animal species, the caudate and putamen nuclei are not distinct. Therefore, they have been largely treated as a similar pair and frequently referred to together as the striatum. There appear to be several parallel "loops" traversing the basal ganglia, beginning with afferents from various areas of the cortex entering and stimulating the main input cells of the striatum. The final output nuclei of the motor loops of the basal ganglia send inhibitory inputs into the movement area of the thalamus, the ventral anterior/ventrolateral (VA/VL) complex (ventral anterior and ventrolateral thalamic nuclei).

Here, we will focus on the role of the basal ganglia in the conscious support of body movement. Recall that conscious actions are guided by plans formulated and relayed by the frontal cortex. Therefore, structures involved in the motoric basal ganglia loops start with afferents from most of the cortex, including sensory and motor areas converging on neurons throughout the caudate and putamen (as stated, usually understood as a group). The subsequent pathways through the basal ganglia can be divided into a direct pathway and an indirect pathway, both of which end up affecting firing of neurons in the VA/VL thalamic complex. The VA/VL complex represents a distinct waystation that specifically targets movement control areas in the cortex, primarily premotor areas. The net effect of activity in the direct pathway is to promote VA/VL firing and therefore planned movement. The net effect of the indirect pathway is to inhibit VA/VL firing and thereby suppress unwanted movement (Figure 10.14). Balance of activity in each of these pathways helps us move how we want, while preventing undesired movements. We will see later how specific diseases impacting each pathway can lead to different symptoms.

A diagram of a coronal slide of a human brain with zoom in showing rough pathways of direct and indirect pathways from striatum to VA/VL complex of thalamus.
Figure 10.14 Direct vs indirect motor pathways The direct pathway of the basal ganglia promotes planned movements, while the indirect pathway inhibits unwanted movements. Purple = GPe/GPi and Green = subthalamic nucleus.

Both the indirect and direct pathway use disinhibition as an important part of how their circuit activity is controlled. To better understand disinhibition, we will look more closely at the direct pathway (see Figure 10.15).

Top: A diagram of a coronal slide of a human brain showing direct pathway from striatum to VA/VL complex of thalamus. Inputs to pathway come from a separate surface view of human brain, showing origins in supplementary motor and premotor areas. Outputs also terminate on cortex surface in similar areas. Bottom: A flow diagram of connections of the direct pathway.
Figure 10.15 Basal ganglia circuits

In the direct pathway, the excitatory input from the frontal cortex areas synapses on inhibitory efferent neurons within the striatum. Those inhibitory striatal neurons then send afferents to the globus pallidus internal (GPi). These GPi neurons are also inhibitory and connect to the VA/VL complex of the thalamus via their own inhibitory efferent neurons. In Figure 10.15, a simplified conscious motor planning diagram for the direct pathway emphasizes the prefrontal cortex because it is where conscious decisions tend to occur, confirming your intended actions and goals along the way. Overall, this inhibition of GPi output neurons results in disinhibition of the movement-related thalamic nuclei in the VA/VL complex. The thalamic cells send excitatory outputs towards the premotor/supplementary motor cortices for body movement, or to the brainstem centers for eye and head movement (Groenewegen, 2003). Planned movement is thereby promoted.

The indirect pathway uses similar processes but has more complex circuitry, involving additional brain nuclei and more inhibitory/disinhibitory connections than in the direct pathway. The end result of exciting the indirect pathway is inhibition of the VA/VL and prevention of overactivation of cortical motor areas or the diminishment of unwanted movements. One structure within the indirect pathway, the subthalamic nucleus, adds an excitation to the equation which increases how much suppression takes place. Malfunctioning here can lead to ballistic thrusting movements and is often blamed for the inappropriate tremors occurring in diseases of the basal ganglia.

Establishing habits and motor sequences

While we've described conscious involvement, the basal ganglia does also maintain heavy involvement in subconscious skill learning and movement control. Sometimes, we have an explicit or conscious desire to repeatedly produce a specific movement sequence that is learned and established via consolidation as an accessible circuit coordinating multiple muscles in a sequence or ensemble. A motor sequence represents a timed pattern of muscle activation sequentially, like finger movements on a piano in a Beethoven concerto. A muscle ensemble represents a group of muscles activated simultaneously to either create a specific movement trajectory or ensure other parts of the body remain stiff while limbs maneuver, like a wrestler using arms, legs, and back contractions to pin an opponent. The basal ganglia are essential for using motor sequences and muscle ensembles in habitual or learned movements. Specifically, the basal ganglia help to select cooperative muscle ensembles and inhibit unhelpful muscles when executing a learned motor skill.

To understand the role of motor system learning, imagine how we might learn to draw cartoon characters like Snoopy or Garfield. Our first tries are often embarrassing. To create these drawings free-handed, we must coordinate the actions of many muscles in our arms and hand. When should we select "this" muscle, or "that," or combinations of both? The basal ganglia circuitry focuses on proper selection of either individual muscles or groups that can produce the desired line in the right direction (drawing Snoopy's rounded snout, or Garfield's stripes or whiskers; see Zham et al., 2017). If the wrong muscles are selected, or the groups of muscles compete rather than cooperate, the drawing will look shaky and wobbly, and we might be likely to discard it rather than showing our friends. After learning to select the right muscle groups for each part, we can proudly show our friends the results. The basal ganglia can select which muscle groups to activate in sequence, so muscle ensembles combine with motor sequences. The package becomes a learned habit, re-played as circumstances demand (piano playing, wrestling).

Some disease states may result from inappropriate activation of motor sequences in basal ganglia circuitry. For example, some researchers suggest that Tourette's syndrome sudden-onset expressions ("tics" or bursts of uncontrolled movement), and dystonia (writhing movements and awkward postures), stem from over-reactive sensitivities at the level of the caudate and putamen, making "chunks" of unintended movements essentially habitual (Mink, 2018; Graybiel and Mink, 2009; Albin and Mink, 2006). The circuitries controlling habitual learned movements are substantially more complex than the conscious motor circuits, involving more sub-structures within the basal ganglia. While we will not describe those complexities here, your author hopes that you will pursue more advanced classes where these circuits are covered, as they are exciting, and hotly debated.

Disorders of the basal ganglia

We shall now describe two devastating diseases, Huntington’s and Parkinson’s, to emphasize functions that rely on the basal ganglia. Some readers may know people who suffer from various stages of movement control deterioration in these disorders. Neither Huntington’s nor Parkinson’s patients can engage proper motor sequences on demand while suffering their deficits (Moisello et al., 2011). Both diseases involve basal ganglia dysfunction, but result in different symptoms due to loss of different parts of the basal ganglia circuitry.

Patients with Parkinson’s disease experience severe, progressive loss of motor control. This includes (1) shakes or tremors; (2) “cogwheel” rigidity, a special kind labeled to emphasize the interspersion of stiffness with short releases, much like clock minute hands clicking into place; and (3) postural instability appearing to derive mostly from insensitivity to vestibular and muscular cues (Sveinbjornsdottir, 2016). While the tremors appear outwardly like other basal ganglia diseases such as Huntington’s, they often manifest more at rest when no effort to engage movement exists (resting tremor), compared to action tremor associated with cerebellar ataxia, discussed later.

Parkinson’s disease is caused by cell death of the dopaminergic neurons originating in the midbrain substantia nigra pars compacta, projecting into the caudate and putamen (Figure 10.16; Fahn 2008).

Top: A horizontal slice of brainstem showing a healthy example substantia nigra (with black cell bodies) on the left and substantia nigra with Parkinson's disease (and very few black cell bodies) on the right. Bottom: Left: a sagittal surface view of a human brain, with major dopaminergic pathways drawn from midbrain regions, projecting to cortex and striatum. Right: a list of the dopaminergic pathways affected by Parkinson's disease. Early/mid: Nigrostriatal; Late: Mesocortical, mesolimbic, tubero-infundibular
Figure 10.16 Parkinson’s disease anatomical pathology

Dopamine contributions to basal ganglia function are complex. As is usually the case with a modulatory transmitter, it largely adjusts the way glutamatergic excitation engages postsynaptic cells. In this case, the postsynaptic cells are inhibitory efferent neurons in the striatum (Surmeier, 2011). The interactions of dopamine with the striatum neurons are complex but their net effect is to stimulate the parts of the caudate and putamen that promote movement behavior, i.e., the direct pathway striatum neurons (follow the circuit diagram in Figure 10.17; Wall et al., 2013, Surmeier, 2011). Its loss in Parkinson’s disease makes movement much more difficult.

Top: A flow diagram of connections of the direct pathway with the substantia nigra pars compacta excitatory input to the caudate/putamen added in. Bottom: A flow diagram of connections of the direct pathway with the substantia nigra pars compacta excitatory input to the caudate/putamen shown as greyed out to represent it's loss in Parkinson's disease. The result is much greater inhibition of the VA/VL complex.
Figure 10.17 Parkinsonian state of basal ganglia

One major treatment for Parkinson’s patients is Levodopa (L-DOPA). It is a precursor molecule of dopamine derived from the amino acid tyrosine and is used by the enzymes within dopaminergic neurons to produce dopamine (see Figure 10.18 and Chapter 3 Basic Neurochemistry).

Diagram of an axonal terminal next to a blood vessel. Precursors are shown being transported out of the blood into the neuron to be converted in to L-DOPA, then in to dopamine.
Figure 10.18 L-DOPA and Parkinson’s disease treatment Reminder: L-DOPA is a precursor in dopamine synthesis.
L-DOPA is also BBB permeable, so L-DOPA in the blood can get into the dopaminergic neuron terminals and boost dopamine synthesis.

When this drug is administered, there is typically an initial surge of dopamine, yielding improved movement. However, sometimes there’s a tendency to overdo. This is seen as impulsivity or a tendency to take more risks behaviorally, but that tapers off as the acute effects diminish (Voon et al., 2017). The greater propensity for movement is typically referred to as an “up, on, or acute” state by comparison to the “down, off, or long-term state” when the L-DOPA wears off (Albin & Leventhal, 2017).

The inconsistency of L-DOPA effects over time is one reason alternative treatments, like stem cell transplantation, are sought for Parkinson’s, which may provide more consistent dopamine presence (Sandstrom et al., 2018). Dopaminergic replacement cell populations derived from stem cells are typically placed into patients’ putamen, because this structure typically receives the highest dopaminergic input, and is most closely associated with movement control (Freed et al., 2011). Also, growth of new axons is considerably challenged across long distances in the adult brain, though strategies for guiding axon growth from the more appropriate substantia nigra pars compacta are being developed (e.g., Zang et al., 2013). Other treatments including a thalamotomy are more radical. In thalamotomy, distinct input regions of the thalamus are targeted for destruction, eliminating the supportive control provided by the compromised basal ganglia. While actors like Michael J. Fox opted for this treatment due to its general preservation of facial expression control, it is not an ultimate cure as the movements produced are often jerky and challenged. Restoring more natural or appropriate basal ganglia activity would be preferable to eliminating its influence, but this remains a challenge to achieve.

Deep brain stimulation is another potential treatment for Parkinson’s disease. In this treatment, electrodes are placed strategically into either the subthalamic nucleus, the internal globus pallidus (GPi) or select regions within the VA/VL thalamus that override the internal signaling strategy provided by the basal ganglia. Deep brain stimulation is particularly helpful in treating the resting tremors that are common in those with Parkinson’s disease. The resting tremors in Parkinson’s disease arise due to complex dysregulation of input to the GPi/VA/VL. Proper placement and high frequency stimulation of electrodes in these areas, supported by power coming from a battery that is typically placed under the skin around the shoulder, can significantly reduce these tremors (Figure 10.19).

Left: Diagram of a human head/torso, with a brain surface view plus a deep brain stimulation device implanted. Right: Coronal brain sections showing DBS leads targeting the VA/VL of thalamus or the globus pallidus, internal segment.
Figure 10.19 Deep brain stimulation and Parkinson’s disease

Huntington’s disease (HD) arises from a dominant genetic mutation on the huntingtin gene within the autosomal portion of chromosome four. Though the genetic mutation is present from conception, the symptoms develop over a lifetime (Walker, 2007). Being autosomal dominant means that a person only requires one copy of the huntingtin gene to develop HD and a parent with HD has a 50% chance of passing on their mutated gene to their offspring. In HD, cognitive difficulties often arise first in the chain of symptoms since the basal ganglia are involved in strategic thinking and developing capacity to adapt thinking to new circumstances, given its interaction with prefrontal and premotor cortices (Novak & Tabrizi, 2011). Typically, movement problems arising in later adulthood, usually around 50-60 years of age, elicit more profound diagnostic concerns.

HD-related movement problems include a sort of undulation of limb and facial movements as if these cannot be suppressed. Limb movements appear dance-like and are referred to as chorea, though they are far from purposefully choreographed or intended (Thompson et al., 1988). At later stages, body movements stiffen, challenging the coordination of swallowing-related pharynx and esophageal muscles (dysphagia). This leads to problems of aspiration and choking (Heemskerk & Roos, 2011).

HD symptoms occur because of the progressive death of striatal neuron cells. The loss of striatal neurons destroys basal ganglia function. The neurons that perish early and more prominently represent those striatal neurons within the indirect pathway, the pathway that primarily suppresses unwanted movement. Progressive deterioration of indirect basal ganglia pathway leaves the VA/VL of the thalamus increasingly disinhibited and yields increased and uncontrolled muscle engagement until the antagonists become overstimulated, stiffening limbs by simultaneous activation (Joel, 2001; Nair et al., 2021). This overstimulation is evidenced as increasingly intense chorea and slowed movement (bradykinesia). Harkening back to drawing Snoopy, consider either unsuppressed wild movements of muscles, or muscles getting locked into tug-of-war battles rather than smoothly guiding the pen. Snoopy's snout would either look too short, or overly long.

There are no directly targeted cures for Huntington's disease right now. The absence of a cure has made diagnosis and genetic testing for Huntington's disease ethically and emotionally complicated. Without clear treatments available, a child with a parent showing symptoms of Huntington's has a dilemma. They have a 50% chance of carrying the mutated gene. If they are tested and have the Huntington's mutation, there is little they can do, so many patients may decide they don't even want to know. Hopefully that dilemma will be resolved in the future, as there are several treatments being actively worked out. Given the disease derives from a distinct mutation in the huntingtin gene, involving an inappropriate expansion of the genetic code, there have been some ideas about either suppressing mutated genes or targeting the mutation with gene therapy for heterozygotes. Transplantation of stem cells into Huntington's patients has also been attempted with varied success (Rosser & Bachoud-Levi, 2012). Currently, however, standard of care is pharmacological treatments, such as tetrabenazine, to reduce symptoms. Tetrabenazine is a vesicular dopamine transporter blocker that prevents the filling of dopaminergic synaptic vesicles (Bonelli, & Wenning, 2006). These drugs suppress chorea because they tend to dampen the movement initiation of the basal ganglia-based motor driving mechanisms. Dopamine net diminishes the activity in the indirect pathway, thereby promoting direct pathway initiation of planned movements. However, these drugs also tend to spread and have high cognitive side effect potential such as depression or other tensions (Paleacu, 2007). Much work is needed to improve treatment of Huntington's disease.

The cerebellum

The cerebellum sits behind and below the cerebrum, atop the brainstem. Its folia look like two lateral bunches of curled-up spaghetti. Its main function is to smooth-out our behavioral efforts. It does this by either pre-processing movement intentions building in the premotor cortical areas, or via upper-level imagined anticipation. For example, if you contemplated a large rock in front of you in the middle of a creek, and whether you might be able to land on it to cross the creek. Alternatively, to improve attempted actions, we process the differences between intended and actual movements, and adjusting signal durations/intensities until proprioception from afferent spinocerebellar tracts says we "nailed it" (Stecina, et al., 2013; Therrien & Bastian, 2019). You might compliment someone with "you have an efficient cerebellum" as a neuroscientist exclusive way of implying they move with sophistication.

How the basal ganglia and cerebellum divide their supportive roles in movement has intrigued neuroscientists for years. The functions of the two different regions are difficult to disentangle (Proville, et al., 2014). To simplify, coordinating necessary muscle selection seems to be the specialization of the basal ganglia, while coordinating contraction timing and degree requires the cerebellum. Analogous smooth and elegant line production while drawing Snoopy requires sophisticated starting and stopping, and smooth transitions of pen direction, to avoid jerky images (see Fujisawa & Okayama, 2015). Another way to think about it is that the basal ganglia "chunk" flexors here and extensors there, while the cerebellum smooths out their actions. Such responsibilities are emphasized more strongly in either structure, though in any one effort we will utilize both structures.

The cerebellum provides its smoothing functions (while you are drawing Snoopy or doing any number of other tasks) through extensive error correction. A motor error occurs when the way we actually move does not match where we intended to move. Detecting errors therefore requires at least 2 streams of information: one about action and the other about intention. Intention comes to the cerebellum from the brain via projections from the pontine nuclei in the brainstem, or from spinal cord regions where descending UMN tracts depict what we intend to do (either consciously or implicitly) to anticipate goals which also ascend the spine into the cerebellum. The cerebellar input about movement derives from proprioception feedback (largely muscle stretch), which depicts moves that happened, ascending from the spinal cord via spinocerebellar tracts (or trigeminal that monitors head/face movement). Progressively, the cerebellum compares these two components and adjusts the prompting UMN signals until we nail it (intended = actual).

The role of the cerebellum in providing smooth integration of motor effort timing and intensity can be appreciated when it is impaired, as happens in cerebellar ataxia or when someone consumes alcohol (Sokolov et al., 2017; Spencer et al., 2005; Bareš et al., 2009). Cerebellar damage (cerebellar ataxia) causes dysmetria, where one exhibits a "drunken gait" because foot placement can't be error-corrected or smoothed, and intention tremor where movements overshoot stopping points and oscillate around target destinations (Schwartze et al., 2016). It is important not to conflate alcohol effects with cerebellar damage. Drunkenness derives from the way alcohol enhances the effects of GABA across wide regions of both motor control and decision-making circuits. Cerebellar damage specifically eliminates the controlled-targeted landing of limbs to the right place. Cerebellar ataxia is analogous to rendering smooth limousine driving into two choices: full throttle or slamming on breaks. Yet visually, walking while drunk and walking with cerebellar ataxia appear outwardly similar. The cerebellum constantly contributes to posture alignment behind the scenes (subconsciously) so removing these contributions causes loose and wavy lack of balance. People can experience ataxic gait for multiple reasons. Any ataxic walking (caused by cerebellar damage or any other source) looks like feet land where they do by accident, rather than on purpose. Cerebellar ataxic gait occurs because the cerebellum can't correct the extent of movements properly, emphasizing approximations (see this video on Ataxia).

Corrective actions incorporated with ongoing movement, not after we perceive movement

Just like the basal ganglia, the cerebellum supports movement control by tweaking the thalamic VA/VL complex, though synapsing through distinct circuits. The cerebellum is inspired into action in relation to initial learning, and later provides anticipatory adjustment signals that integrate into ongoing movement but are sent prior to or in conjunction with the UMN signals that descend to initiate LMN actions. Most cerebellum contributions are made subconsciously and become integrated into movements we subsequently produce (the lack of sufficient adjustment is shown when errors occur, such as ataxia). Success in generating desired smooth movements, ending without jerky shakes, requires error corrections to be incorporated before movement begins. If we had to wait for proprioceptive feedback before correcting, there would always be a “jerk-back” effect due to delays inherent in sensory systems. This can result from conscious movement monitoring. Conscious proprioception (cortical) is slower than ongoing movements, so modifications come post-movement, making it jerky and uncoordinated. Damaged cerebellum patients are limited to this, so when their conscious stream is distracted, they produce more movement errors because their cortex cannot multitask well (Brunamonti, et al., 2014). Thus, a well-primed (skill consolidated) healthy cerebellum recognizes the need, then delivers its adjustments along with subsequent UMN-driven movement efforts rather than correcting errors after the fact. It learns while we do behind the scenes to determine how much boosting or tapering is necessary, and subsequently contributes these adjustments to future movements. Imagine these contributions behind the scenes of established Olympic floor moves, or the slight-of-hand demonstrated by a seasoned magician (Dahms et al., 2020).

Practicing and imagining success

It’s important for new behavior to start off slowly, allowing sufficient time for proprioceptive sensory feedback to reach the CNS and become conscious perception. We do this all the time when we are learning how to move in specific ways. For example, choreographed fight moves à la Jackie Chan are typically rehearsed slowly before filming so actors can anticipate blows and pull back to “act” hit. By going slowly, we have time to consciously perceive motor errors and use our cerebellar processes to correct them. Through this slow practice, the cerebellum helps us learn how to execute movements faster and faster, with less conscious perception of proprioception needed to keep our bodies on track. Notably, even imagined movements can play a role in this learning—a mental rehearsal without physical engagement (Decety & Ingvar, 1990, Jackson et al., 2003). Actors, dancers, skaters, and sports professionals do this all the time. The performance improvements from imagined practice are real, not illusory, as they entrain the cerebellar circuitry support of cerebral movement control via the same circuitry that adapts to actual movement.

Primary motor cortex

The primary motor cortex (M1) region receives our “upper-level” synaptic progression last. This progression collects and coordinates movement intentions before sending those UMN compilations down to the brainstem and spinal cord for movement generation by the LMNs. M1 is also the first cortical area with full lateralization of limb and face movement control: M1 neurons on the right side of the brain all target LMNs for the left limbs and facial area, while M1 neurons on the left side of the brain all target LMNs for the right limbs and facial areas. This lateralization contrasts with the premotor regions we discussed so far, which engage bilateral control. M1 lateralization is restricted to limbs and facial lateral areas, not the trunk, which is generally ipsilaterally controlled.

Primary Precentral Gyrus Organization

The posterior frontal cortex has a major crease, the central sulcus, dividing the precentral (green in Figure 10.20) and postcentral gyri.

Left top: Lateral surface view of human brain with primary motor cortex highlighted. Left bottom: Sagittal surface view of human brain with primary motor coretx highlighted. Right: Coronal view of major divisions of primary motor cortex, showing drawings of represented body parts lying along the surface of the cortex.
Figure 10.20 Primary motor cortex

This strip, from between the hemispheres out laterally, ending just above the lateral fissure (defining the top of the temporal lobe), contains the primary drivers of voluntarily organized/coordinated movement known collectively as the primary motor cortex. The neurons of the primary motor cortex are arranged in a homunculus. This arrangement is essentially a map of a little person lying across the precentral gyrus, so toe control is buried between the hemispheres. This map was famously created by neurosurgeon Wilder Penfield, who assessed movements when brain regions were stimulated during surgeries correcting epilepsy and made direct recordings during movements (Penfield and Rasmussen, 1950). The idea of a homunculus depicts the “person” as a distorted image made up of enlarged areas where more neuronal space engages with specific moveable body parts. The complexity or subtlety of movement of any body part tends to engage larger numbers of neurons as this attribute goes up, so the eventual map of a person has a large tongue, huge lips, prominent face around the forehead, huge hands and feet, with a smaller torso, limbs, and joints, according to the degree of movement sophistication possible. The motor homunculus is to movement sophistication as the somatosensory homunculus is to somatosensory density (see Chapter 9 Touch and Pain).

Descending motor projections

The primary motor cortex neurons send their axons out to the brainstem and spinal cord via three tracts (see Figure 10.21).

Upper left inset: Coronal view of major divisions of primary motor cortex, showing drawings of represented body parts lying along the surface of the cortex. Major neuronal pathways are shown projecting down to the spinal cord. Rest of diagram: Neural pathway diagram, showing brain original of corticospinal tracts, projecting down through the spinal cord, then synapsing on motor neuron in the ventral spinal cord grey. From there, lower motor neurons project out of the ventral spinal cord roots, towards the periphery.
Figure 10.21 Corticospinal motor pathways for the body
  • The corticobulbar (or corticonuclear) tract, carries descending efferents from the cortex, synapsing on brainstem nuclei serving regions of the jaw, face, ear mechanisms, for gagging or swallowing, or crinkling our noses. Corticobulbar tracts largely provide bilateral support, where each side directly serves ipsilaterally, and crosses over at the level of various nuclei to the contralateral side. The exception to this rule would be the descending tracts subserving the facial nucleus, specifically the lower part of the face, which is exclusively contralateral (Patestas & Gartner, 2016). We're overlooking autonomic patterning aspects of this projection to focus on controlled movement.
  • The lateral corticospinal tract is the major descending tract for body limbs. It crosses almost entirely to the other side of the body to control the contralateral (opposite side from originating hemisphere) limbs, mostly the arm/hand/fingers and leg/foot/toes.
  • The anterior corticospinal tract is a smaller but important tract responsible for controlling trunk muscles and more medial aspects. While some of this tract decussates (crosses over) at the level of the spinal cord in the ventral white matter as shown in Figure 10.21, a majority subserves the ipsilateral side.

The upper part of our body (head) is largely controlled via cranial nerves derived from the brainstem and therefore the corticobulbar tract. The two corticospinal tracts descend to different levels of the spinal cord. Within the spinal cord, these tracts interact with LMNs and the circuitry making up the reflex architecture (also used to engage the knee-jerk, or withdrawal reflexes, utilizing local spinal circuitry described above) to control the lower parts of our bodies such as limbs and trunk.

Why do some descending UMNs control need to cross over to operate the opposite side of the body? One simple explanation may be that visual information passing through lenses of your eyes is flipped (see Chapter 6 Vision). Events occurring to our left pass to the right sides of our retinae. The right sides of each retina connect with the right hemisphere, while the left connects with the left. However, the information they process is relevant to the opposite sides of our bodies. This probably created pressure for natural selection to derive a map of limb control so each hemisphere could control the opposite side.

People behind the science: Vector Trajectories

Apostolos Georgopoulos MD, Ph.D. currently heads a Brain Center at The University of Minnesota where he is Regents Professor of Neuroscience. His work brought important insights to the way motor cortex activates limb movement. To appreciate his findings, it’s important to understand the concept of a vector, a force directing movement encompassing both speed and direction.

When we kick a ball softly, its path forms a vector with little speed. Kicking the ball harder in the same direction maintains this vector with greater speed. A different direction would elicit a different vector. Similarly with moving our arms. The generation of force accumulation to drive movement intensity at the LMN level was discussed above in 10.1 The Physiological Actions Implementing Movement – Contraction of Muscles.

The key to Georgopoulos' findings is, across the population of M1 neurons, each neuron's activity seems to be associated with a vector of limb movement. Or, as each M1 motoneuron fires action potentials, their frequencies intensify in relation to the limb movement's direction and trajectory range (Georgopoulos et al., 1983).

To determine this, Dr. Georgopoulos and his team recorded from hundreds of individual M1 neurons within the brains of monkeys while the monkeys moved a specialized apparatus (Georgopoulos et al., 1992). It was set up like a large lever-shift which picked up movement direction, and could be adjusted to provide back-pressure along certain trajectories.

Think of this as a large old-school Atari-like joystick, shifting rather than simply angling in different directions. When the animal pushed the lever in one direction, a distinct neuron population appearing to "prefer" that direction elicited strong bursts of activity, while other populations "preferring" other directions diminished responses. If the vector of an M1 neuron's preference required extra force, that MI neuron increased its firing rate (Georgopoulos et al., 1982). The key to the developing hypothesis was at each motion of the arm, larger populations of neurons contributed their "vote" for arm vectors into the descending feed. At each moment key populations "won" the vote to over-ride alternative movement preferences. Brain democracy!

At first, these experiments were performed with only two-dimensional movement directions across a flat surface (1983). Eventually, more exciting results were obtained from monkey arm movement tracked three-dimensionally. Based on the same premise, a more complex division of "preferences" yielded similar results (Georgopoulos et al., 1988). It is fascinating to consider the wider implications of this. Guiding one's arm or hand towards a target follows calculations of vector trajectories in regions like the posterior parietal cortex and the cerebellum, to be sent forward toward the premotor cortex.

It's now clearer how this guidance appears to help reinforce individual neurons contributing "votes" or preference activations in concert with needed directions through increasingly selective intensities of stimulation of select M1 cortical neurons (e.g., Georgopoulos et al., 1992; Taira et al., 1990). Since these preference activations persist across a population, this also seems to provide greater stability in the capacity to elicit distinct movements, diminishing the probability of select damage eliminating specific movement trajectory capacities. This would be incredibly helpful in restoring post-stroke movement, for example. To draw Snoopy, many hand muscles need to be selected individually or simultaneously and this neuronal "voting" is also supported by basal ganglia contributions along the way when we draw him again, and again (necessary for moving pictures).

Moving Beyond Vector Trajectories into Controlling Robot Arms

In the previous section, we discussed how the pioneering work of Georgopoulos pointed towards this key cortical element of “voting populations.” More recently, new and fascinating cutting-edge things have been happening to build off his work. By studying consensus of neuronal activities and how they link to vector trajectories, researchers have begun to be able to predict where limbs might go based on neuronal activity! Once you can predict what neuronal activity does, it becomes possible to tap in to those neurons to recreate movements. This basic idea is what underpins many modern efforts to link robot prosthetics to the human brain.

Many forms of brain or spinal cord damage can render a person unable to move any limbs despite owning an active and intact brain (quadriplegic). Neuroscientists such as Andrew Schwartz and others have been working to turn that remaining ability to direct movements in the brain into real movements of robotic prosthetic devices. This work is all based in the original concepts derived from Georgopoulos and has now yielded robotic arms that can respond to recordings from a patient’s brain and engage complex arm and hand type movements to select, grasp, and move objects! The details of how two human subjects with quadriplegia were implanted with specialized electrodes over the arm-related region of their M1 homunculi and subsequently gained considerable control over an attached robot arm are originally described in Lancet (Collinger et al., 2013). Since this initial proof-of-principle, improvements have been made. For example, a patient in 2015 received a version of this system that was not just unidirectional—taking motor commands and linking them to the prosthetic. This newer version also incorporated input from sensory information. Remember back to our example of a monkey swinging through the trees. To swing, grasp and release well, the monkey integrated several sources of touch information. Grasping without feeling the grasp would make branch swinging a far greater challenge for the monkey. The same applies to patients with prosthetic limbs. Adding touch feedback greatly improved the ability of the patient to use their limb. These devices, adjusted for circumstances, are moving outside of a theoretical possibility into a distinct therapeutic option reality! If movement practice feedback could also be incorporated, it is conceivable that practiced learning could get such patients back to cooking in the kitchen!

A Special inspiration from your author

It has been a pleasure sharing a neuroscientist’s view of movement with you all. All the complexity ensures our capacity to do what we want to do. Considerable control has evolved at both lower implementation levels and higher formulation levels. Collaborations between brain, spine, and muscles above help us repeatedly do what we want without having to re-invent every motion from scratch. Background processing and delegation promotes the feeling of quick shifts from thinking to doing. Now you should have a better sense of the complexity. On several occasions, this chapter has used expressions like 'apparently,' 'it seems like,' or 'seems to.' Such expressions demonstrate there's a lot neuroscientists still need to learn about body movement mechanisms. The author hopes that future neuroscientists or readers with related intentions may see clarifying this as a challenge. Also, this author wishes to thank the editor Dr. Elizabeth Kirby, his father (Dr. Harald M. Sandstrom), his brother (Jonathan R. Sandstrom), and his colleague Dr. Gary L. Dunbar for their extraordinary editorial support that substantially improved this chapter’s clarity and focus.

Citation/Attribution

This book may not be used in the training of large language models or otherwise be ingested into large language models or generative AI offerings without OpenStax's permission.

Want to cite, share, or modify this book? This book uses the Creative Commons Attribution License and you must attribute OpenStax.

Attribution information
  • If you are redistributing all or part of this book in a print format, then you must include on every physical page the following attribution:
    Access for free at https://openstax.org/books/introduction-behavioral-neuroscience/pages/1-introduction
  • If you are redistributing all or part of this book in a digital format, then you must include on every digital page view the following attribution:
    Access for free at https://openstax.org/books/introduction-behavioral-neuroscience/pages/1-introduction
Citation information

© Oct 30, 2024 OpenStax. Textbook content produced by OpenStax is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License . The OpenStax name, OpenStax logo, OpenStax book covers, OpenStax CNX name, and OpenStax CNX logo are not subject to the Creative Commons license and may not be reproduced without the prior and express written consent of Rice University.