Skip to ContentGo to accessibility pageKeyboard shortcuts menu
OpenStax Logo

A micrograph shows the layers of nerve cells in the retina. The top layer (green) is made up of cells called photoreceptors that convert light into electrical signals to relay to the brain.
Figure 6.1 Vision begins in the retina, a layered structure where photoreceptors absorb light to start a chain of activity that ends in the brain. Image Credit: Wei Li, National Eye Institute, National Institutes of Health. Public domain

Meet the Author

Richard Olivo, PhD

Meet the author

For most people, vision is not only their most important sense, it is the sensory system that defines the world around them. If we weren’t such visual animals, humans wouldn’t move easily through the space around us, recognize other people, identify familiar objects, and do numerous other tasks that seem effortless. Movies and videos wouldn’t be persuasive representations of the world. Our visual system acts so immediately and fluently that it is difficult for us to realize that constructing the visual world is an extremely difficult task that our brain accomplishes through multiple steps that begin in the eye and continue in major regions of the cerebral cortex. Our goal in this chapter will be to follow those steps, beginning in the retina and proceeding through the primary visual cortex and adjacent areas in the occipital lobe, and continuing in the inferotemporal cortex. We will first encounter photoreceptors in the retina, specialized neural cells that catch light energy, and we will end with cortical neurons that respond to faces, places, and complex visual shapes.

An analogy can help us understand how unlikely vision would seem to us if we didn’t have a sophisticated visual system ourselves. In the 1940s, scientists discovered how bats could fly freely in the dark and catch flying insects without using vision. The bats use echolocation: they emit ultrasonic screeches and then detect the echoes of sounds reflected from tree branches and their insect prey. It’s hard to imagine what it might be like to perceive surroundings through echolocation, but our vision does something similar. The sun or artificial sources flood our surroundings with light, and we detect reflections of that light from the surfaces around us. Our brain processes that sensory information to construct a mental image of the world—something we might not believe could be done if we didn’t do it ourselves.

Citation/Attribution

This book may not be used in the training of large language models or otherwise be ingested into large language models or generative AI offerings without OpenStax's permission.

Want to cite, share, or modify this book? This book uses the Creative Commons Attribution License and you must attribute OpenStax.

Attribution information
  • If you are redistributing all or part of this book in a print format, then you must include on every physical page the following attribution:
    Access for free at https://openstax.org/books/introduction-behavioral-neuroscience/pages/1-introduction
  • If you are redistributing all or part of this book in a digital format, then you must include on every digital page view the following attribution:
    Access for free at https://openstax.org/books/introduction-behavioral-neuroscience/pages/1-introduction
Citation information

© Oct 30, 2024 OpenStax. Textbook content produced by OpenStax is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License . The OpenStax name, OpenStax logo, OpenStax book covers, OpenStax CNX name, and OpenStax CNX logo are not subject to the Creative Commons license and may not be reproduced without the prior and express written consent of Rice University.