Learning Objectives
By the end of this section, you will be able to:
- Describe infants’ reflexes
- Identify the typical milestones of motor development
- Distinguish between gross and fine motor development
- Explain influences on motor development in infancy and toddlerhood
Mateo and Jonas brought their eight-month-old daughter Bella to her pediatrician. Bella has been sitting independently for a couple of months and tries to pull herself up to a standing position, but she shows no interest in crawling. The pediatrician says there is no reason for concern. There are many important milestones in physical development, she says, but crawling is not one of them. This is quite surprising news to both dads. In fact, some babies never crawl: they go straight from sitting to walking. Others scoot on their bottoms, “commando” crawl on their bellies, or adopt a “crab crawl,” placing their weight on one leg while bending the other to propel themselves forward. The pediatrician says that not crawling will not prevent Bella from reaching other important physical milestones like standing and walking.
The first two years of life bring dramatic gains in the development of movement and motor skills. A newborn’s involuntary movements transform to the infant’s ability to exert some limited control over their muscles. Suddenly, that infant is a two-year-old child who loves running, jumping, and climbing.
Newborn Reflexes
Babies are born with several reflexes (Table 3.5). Some, like coughing, sneezing, and blinking, stay with us, while others, like rooting, disappear.
Reflex | Description | Average Age at Which Reflex Disappears |
---|---|---|
Stepping | Makes stepping motion when sole of feet touches hard surface | 2 months |
Moro | Arms fling sideways with palms up as if in falling motion | 2–4 months |
Rooting | Turns head to side, opens mouth wide, and makes sucking motions when stroked on cheek | 3–4 months |
Palmar grasp | Grasps a finger placed in palm | 3–6 months |
Swimming | Kicks and makes swimming motions if placed face-down in water | 4–6 months |
Sucking | Sucks when something is in mouth | 4 months |
Tonic neck | Assumes fencer stance when relaxed and lying face up if someone turns head to side | 6 months |
Plantar | Curls toes in when finger is placed below them | 9–12 months |
Babinski | Toes fan out when foot is stroked | 12 months |
Blinking | Blinks when eyes touch something or are exposed to sudden brightness | Stays |
Coughing | Coughs when airway is stimulated | Stays |
Gagging | Gags when back of mouth/throat is stimulated | Stays |
Sneezing | Sneezes when nasal passage stimulated | Stays |
Why do newborns have reflexes? Some, such as the sucking reflex for nursing or bottle feeding, are for survival. Others are a stress response connected to the sympathetic nervous system. For example, the Moro reflex, also called the startle reflex, is activated when the infant is startled by a sudden movement or noise. The head automatically moves back, and the arms and legs flail out. Other reflexes do not have clear functions but are appealing to see, like the Babinski and plantar reflexes. If you glide your finger on the inner sole of the infant’s foot beneath their toes, the toes curl up, but if you stroke the outer sole, the toes spread out (Figure 3.12).
Some reflexes may be absent in newborns who have neurological impairments or who were born preterm, for example, while others may remain beyond the typical age of disappearance. Health-care professionals are likely to check for the appearance and disappearance of various reflexes to better understand how infants are developing and provide early intervention when needed (Swapna et al., 2020). This scan often begins with the Apgar test immediately after birth.
Milestones in Motor Development
Motor development milestones in the first two years of life are rapid and dramatic, and parents and caregivers often eagerly anticipate their child’s learning to sit, stand, and walk. Each achievement occurs during a given time span. The average age for walking is twelve months, for example, but infants can begin to walk as early as eight months or as late as seventeen to eighteen months. These milestones follow the predictable sequence of cephalocaudal and proximodistal development. For example, following the cephalocaudal pattern, infants are first able to hold their heads up, then sit, pull up, and finally walk. In the proximodistal pattern, infants can grab an object with their full hand before they can manipulate it with individual fingers.
Gross Motor Skills
Voluntary movements that rely on large muscle groups and typically activate the arms, legs, head, and torso are gross motor skills. Infants reach several major postural milestones in the first year (Table 3.6). Movement skills also typically build on one another. For example, after crawling, infants may pull themselves up and then gain the strength to walk. If a child is lagging on one or more milestones, the delay can be addressed through early intervention and monitored by a pediatrician. However, motor milestones also show cultural, experiential, and individual variability (Adolph & Hoch, 2019).
Muscle Control Milestone | Age Range Expected |
---|---|
Supports head | 6–8 weeks old |
Lifts head and chest when on tummy | 2–5 months |
Rolls from tummy to back | 4–6 months |
Sits alone (unsupported) | 4–8 months |
Stands with support | 5–12 months |
Stands without support | 7–17 months |
Walks with support | 6–13 months |
Walks without support | 8–17 months |
Link to Learning
During the first few months of life, infants are rapidly moving from one gross motor milestone to the next. This video shows some of the motor milestones they reach during the first three months.
Fine Motor Skills
Although gross motor skills typically appear first, infants and toddlers are also beginning to develop fine motor skills, coordinated movements performed by small muscles to manipulate and control objects or perform precise tasks like reaching for and grasping an object (Table 3.7). Finger movements readily come to mind, but the toes, eyes, face, tongue, and lips also execute fine motor movements.
Fine Motor Skill Milestone | Average Age Expected |
---|---|
Opens hands briefly | 2 months |
Holds a toy when put in their hand | 4 months |
Brings hand to mouth | 4 months |
Reaches to grab toy | 6 months |
Bangs objects together | 9 months |
Transfers objects between hands | 9 months |
Grasps with two fingers | 12 months |
Feeds self with fingers | 18 months |
Tries to use buttons or switches on a toy | 24 months |
Like gross motor skills, fine motor skills often develop in a predictable sequence. At about four months of age, the infant uses two hands to reach for an object and then with more experience uses only one arm. Grasping begins with hands and palms (no thumb), a skill called the ulnar grasp. At about six months, the palmar grasp reflex is replaced by voluntary grasping. Infants begin to use their thumbs at around nine months of age when they grasp with their forefinger and thumb (Figure 3.13), dramatically improving the ability to control and manipulate objects. These coordinated actions are quite an accomplishment in only a few months’ time.
The development of fine motor skills improves with practice, as you can observe if you watch an infant pick up objects and put them into a container repeatedly. With practice, infants will change their grasping patterns to fit the shape and size of the objects (Barrett et al., 2008). Fine motor skills, much like gross motor skills, also show variability based on family environment, culture, and opportunities to practice various movements (Hoch et al., 2024). For example, being in a home with more access to toys that call upon fine motor skills predicts improved fine motor skills.
Motor skills are also the result of interactions with other domains of development. For example, infants must plan and use their senses to move about in their environment. The ability to speak is also dependent on their being able to control the muscles in their mouth and larynx. In addition, although motor skills may appear to develop as individual skills, they build on each other, with each new one serving as the foundation for the next. As infants get older, individual motor skills combine into more complex skills, allowing infants to explore and learn about their surroundings.
Link to Learning
The development of fine motor skills during infancy allows infants to feed themselves and explore their environment. Many of these skills are learned through play.
Motor Development as a Dynamic System
One theory that has helped developmental psychologists understand the complexities of motor development is dynamic systems theory, which posits that developmental behaviors and milestones are the result of interactions between systems, including those within the individual. According to the original theorist, Esther Thelen, motor development is the result of interactions between internal developmental domains (e.g., cognition and the senses), maturation, and the environment (Fujihira & Taga, 2023; Thelen & Smith, 2007). For example, to walk, a child must have several coordinated and developed internal systems including balance, leg muscles, stepping movements, coordination, vision, and spatial awareness (Thelen & Ulrich, 1991).
From a dynamic systems perspective, every motor skill results from multiple maturational and environmental processes. For example, growth in the cerebellum, which contributes to balance and coordination, myelination of the nervous system, and synaptic pruning are critical for acquiring both fine and gross motor skills (Morizawa et al., 2022; Tiemeier et al., 2010). Gross motor skills are also influenced by genetics. Identical twins, who have nearly identical genetic makeup, are more similar in the timing of their motor milestones than fraternal twins, who share around half of the same genes (Ooki, 2006). However, even identical twins have unique experiences that can result in very different motor skill capabilities, as developmental psychologist and neurobiologist Myrtle McGraw observed in case study experiments in the 1930s (McGraw, 1935; Parnin, 2022).
Dynamic systems theory has since expanded into a dynamic systems approach, applying the theory to other domains of development over the lifespan, including cognition and language (Fogel, 2011; Spencer et al., 2011). You may notice that this approach incorporates many of the theories you learned about in Chapter 1 Lifespan Psychology and Developmental Theories, such as cognitive development and behavioral genetics.
Link to Learning
Review this video to observe some of the original footage of Myrtle McGraw’s twin study of motor development.
Experience and Motor Development
While the dynamic systems approach improves our understanding of the influences of systems within the individual, ecological systems and sociocultural theories inform our view of the factors outside the individual that shape developmental outcomes. These can come from cultural and familial environments as well as from individual play behaviors and resources.
Although the sequence of motor milestones is expected to be similar for all children, the timing and abilities can vary widely, demonstrating that environmental and cultural factors play an important role (Adolph & Hoch, 2019). For example, in Tajikistan, infants are regularly swaddled and placed in gahvora cradles for long periods of time. Because of their temporary immobility, these infants reach motor milestones later (Karasik et al., 2018).
In Northern China, where infants lie on their backs with their body movement restricted by being buried in a bag of fine sand for most of the day for toileting practice, the onset of sitting, crawling, and walking is delayed by several months (Mei, 1994). In other cultures, such as in the United States, Argentina, South Korea, and Italy, infants spend a lot of time sitting with postural support from furniture (United States and Argentina) or their mother’s arms (South Korea and Italy) (Karasik et al., 2015). In contrast, infants in Western Kenya have more practice with both supported and independent sitting, which may allow them to sit independently somewhat earlier than in cultures where infants are provided with a lot of support while sitting (Karasik et al., 2015).
Crawling and walking also show great variability based on culture and parenting beliefs. In some cultures, infants skip crawling or walk before they crawl, based on factors like maternal handling, cultural beliefs, and whether parents encourage or discourage crawling (Adolph et al., 2018; Super, 1976). Parenting beliefs can also influence the way they handle their infants. In some cultures, caregivers perform exercises to encourage motor development that might be viewed as rough in another culture (Adolph et al., 2018).
In another example, research following the Back to Sleep campaign has shown that infants placed on their backs while sleeping reach some motor milestones significantly later than infants who slept on their stomachs (Brouwer et al., 2006; Johnson, 2004). In response, pediatricians began encouraging parents to give their infants “tummy time,” which is associated with earlier development of gross motor skills such as walking, standing, and sitting (Hewitt et al., 2020).
Play
Play uses motor skills and cognitive skills, both connected to brain development. For example, with time and practice, the infant who begins by grasping a spoon is soon able to use it as a tool for eating. Besides the physical ability required to do this, the child also uses cognitive skills to play. Using paintbrushes, crayons, balls, and blocks in play takes similar planning and coordination. By one year, the child can point to objects of interest, use more motor skills to explore environments, and interact more with others during playtime. At eighteen months, the child may seek interaction, enjoy patty-cake, and clap during play. Parents and caregivers can support motor development through play by providing age-appropriate toys and activities and encouraging exploration and movement in a safe, supportive environment (Table 3.8).
Play Activity | Motor Development | Age Range |
---|---|---|
Tummy time | Spending supervised time on their tummy can help infants strengthen neck and upper body muscles. | Infancy |
Baby gym | Playing under a play gym with hanging toys encourages babies to reach and grasp. | Infancy |
Mirror play | Playing with a baby-safe mirror encourages visual tracking and body awareness. | 6 months–2 years |
Crawling tunnel | Crawling through a toy tunnel promotes mobility and exploration. | 6 months–1 year |
Puzzles and building blocks | Interacting with age-appropriate puzzles and soft building blocks improves problem-solving skills, hand-eye coordination, and gross and fine motor skills. | 6 months–2 years |
Dance and music | Moving to music strengthens balance and coordination. | 6 months–2 years |
Arts and crafts | Finger painting, coloring, and cutting with safety scissors promote fine motor skills and creativity. | 1–2 years |
Obstacle courses | Engaging with a simple obstacle course with cushions, pillows, and tunnels encourages crawling, climbing, and balance. | 1–2 years |
Puppet shows | Interacting with puppets enhances visual tracking and fine motor skills. | 1–2 years |
Play starts in infancy and can inform several aspects of early development including motor skills, social interaction, self-exploration, and exploration of the environment. Play also helps caregivers and health-care providers observe how well development is progressing.
References
Adolph K. E., & Franchak J. M. (2017). The development of motor behavior. WIREs Cognitive Science, 8(1-2), e1430. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1430
Adolph, K. E., & Hoch, J. E. (2019). Motor development: Embodied, embedded, enculturated, and enabling. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 141–164. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-102836
Adolph, K. E., Hoch, J. E., & Cole, W. G. (2018). Development (of walking): 15 suggestions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(8), 699–711. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.05.010
Barrett, T. M., Traupman, E., & Needham, A. (2008). Infants’ visual anticipation of object structure in grasp planning. Infant Behavior and Development, 31(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/jinfbeh.2007.05.004
Brouwer, S. I., van Beijsterveldt, T. C., Bartels, M., Hudziak, J. J., & Boomsma, D. I. (2006). Influences on achieving motor milestones: A twin/singleton study. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9(3), 424–430. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.9.3.424
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2024, May 8). CDC’s developmental milestones. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/actearly/milestones/index.html
Fogel, A. (2011). Theoretical and applied dynamic systems research in developmental science. Child Development Perspectives, 5(4), 267–272. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00174.x
Fujihira, R., & Taga, G. (2023). Dynamical systems model of development of the action differentiation in early infancy: A requisite of physical agency. Biological Cybernetics, 117, 81–93. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00422-023-00955-y
Heidenreich, S. (2021, May 6). Understanding primitive reflexes: How they impact child development and intervention strategies for integration. OccupationalTherapy.com, Article 5409. https://www.occupationaltherapy.com/articles/understanding-primitive-reflexes-they-impact-5409-5409
Hewitt, L., Kerr, R., Stanley, R.M., & Okely, A. D. (2020). Tummy time and infant health outcomes: A systematic review. Pediatrics, 145(6), Article e20192168. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2019-2168
Hoch, J., Hospodar, C., Koch da Costa Aguiar Alves, G., & Adolph, K. (2024). Variations in infants’ physical and social environments shape spontaneous locomotion. Developmental Psychology, 60(6) 991–1001. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0001745
Johnson, K. (2004). “Back to sleep” cuts tummy time; still vital to put babies on back to sleep. Pediatric News, 38, 1–3.
Karasik, L. B., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Ossmy, O., & Adolph, K. E. (2018). The ties that bind: Cradling in Tajikistan. PLoS ONE, 13(10), Article e0204428. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0204428
Karasik, L. B., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Adolph, K. E., & Bornstein, M. H. (2015). Places and postures: A cross-cultural comparison of sitting in 5-month-olds. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46(8), 1023–1038. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0022022115593803
McGraw, M. B. (1935). Growth: a study of Johnny and Jimmy. Appleton-Century.
Mei, J. (1994). The Northern Chinese custom of rearing babies in sandbags: Implications for motor and intellectual development. In J. H. A. van Rossum & J. I. Laszlo (Eds.), Motor development: Aspects of normal and delayed development (pp. 41–48). Uitgeverij.
Morizawa, Y. M., Matsumoto, M., Nakashima, Y., Endo, N., Aida, T., Ishikane, H., Beppu, K., Moritoh, S., Inada, H., Osumi, N., Shigetomi, E., Koizumi, S., Yang, G., Hirai, H., Tanaka, K., Tanaka, K. F., Ohno, N., Fukazawa, Y., & Matsui, K. (2022). Synaptic pruning through glial synapse engulfment upon motor learning. Nature neuroscience, 25(11), 1458–1469. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-022-01184-5
Noritz, G. H., Murphy, N. A., & Neuromotor Screening Expert Panel (2013). Motor delays: Early identification and evaluation. Pediatrics, 131(6), e2016–e2027. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-1056
Ooki, S. (2006). Motor development of Japanese twins in childhood as reported by mothers. Environmental Health and Preventive Medicine, 11, 55–64. https://doi.org/10.1265/ehpm.11.55
Parnin, B. (2022, January 20). Experimenting with babies: The Myrtle McGraw collection. Indiana University Bloomington. https://blogs.libraries.indiana.edu/filmarch/2022/01/20/experimenting-with-babies-the-dr-myrtle-mcgraw-collection/
Spencer, J. P., Perone, S., & Buss, A. T. (2011). Twenty years and going strong: A dynamic systems revolution in motor and cognitive development. Child development perspectives, 5(4), 260–266. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00194.x
Super, C. M. (1976). Environmental effects on motor development: The case of “African infant precocity”. Developmental Medical Child Neurology, 18(5), 561–567. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.1976.tb04202.x
Swapna, N., Kumar, P., Kalam, B. R., Anju, V. A., & Arunraj, K. (2020). Diagnostic relevance of primitive reflexes in high-risk newborns: A systematic review. Journal of Indian Speech Language & Hearing Association, 34(1), 24–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.4103/jisha.JISHA_22_19
Thelen, E. & Smith, L. B. (2007). Dynamic systems theory (Vol. 1): Theoretical models of human development. Handbook of Child Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0106
Thelen, E., & Ulrich, B. D. (1991). Hidden skills: A dynamic systems analysis of treadmill stepping during the first year. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 56(1). Article 104. https://doi.org/10.2307/1166099
Tiemeier, H., Lenroot, R. K., Greenstein, D. K., Tran, L., Pierson, R., & Giedd, J. N. (2010). Cerebellum development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal morphometric MRI study. Neuroimage, 49(1), 63 –70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.016
WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study Group, & de Onis, M. (2006). Assessment of sex differences and heterogeneity in motor milestone attainment among populations in the WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study [Supplemental material]. Acta Paediatrica, 95(S450), 66–75. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.2006.tb02377.x