Skip to ContentGo to accessibility pageKeyboard shortcuts menu
OpenStax Logo
Lifespan Development

16.1 The Meaning of Aging in Late Adulthood

Lifespan Development16.1 The Meaning of Aging in Late Adulthood

Learning Objectives

By the end of this section, you will be able to:

  • Describe the social and cultural aspects of aging
  • Discuss aging stereotypes
  • Explain Erikson’s integrity versus despair challenge
  • Describe self and socioemotional development in late adulthood

Reina is eighty-five years old. When her children ask what she’s been up to recently, she gives them a lengthy report of her activities, which include making 10 pints of guava jam, tutoring her new neighbor in Spanish, buying Three Kings Day gifts for her grandchildren, having lunch with friends, attending mass, and rooting for her favorite soccer team, Club América (who are apparently not playing up to Reina’s standards).

Research shows that older adults tend to be happier than people in younger age groups, and many maintain their independence for most of their later years. This section focuses on different perspectives on older age, including stereotypes and discrimination. It also covers how our sense of self changes later in life and emotional shifts that typically occur as we get older.

Perspectives on Aging

Older adults typically have a different outlook on life than younger individuals. Their knowledge of their mortality and the realization that time is more limited can drive several changes related to reaching health and fitness goals, nurturing relationships, and maintaining a positive emotional state (Keldal & Şeker, 2021; Strout et al., 2018).

Different cultures also have varied perspectives on age (Figure 16.2). Historically, cultures in eastern Asia were described as having more positive views of aging and viewing aging as a symbol of higher status, due to cultural principles that valued wisdom, collectivism, and family (Gire, 2019; Kornadt et al., 2022). However, more recent research suggests cultural differences are more complex than initially thought. In one study, members of Eastern cultures report seeing older adults as more competent than younger adults but also having more negative feelings toward them and a higher likelihood of avoiding contact (Vauclair et al., 2016).

A group gathers around a board game.
Figure 16.2 The way different cultures view older adults may depend on cultural values as well as economic conditions. (credit: modification of work “Senior citizen” by Aan Kasman/Flickr, CC BY 2.0)

This more nuanced view was supported by a large-scale study of participants from twenty-six Asian and Western countries. The participants tended to have similar views of aging and consistently associated aging with greater wisdom, knowledge, and respect from others, but also with decreased physical ability, attractiveness, and ability to learn new things. However, views of aging were predicted not by culture but by the percentage of older adults in the population, and higher percentages correlated with more negative views (Löckenhoff et al., 2009; Wettstein et al., 2024)1 (Figure 16.3). Many of these countries also had low socioeconomic status (SES); thus, a potential explanation is that older adults are viewed as competitors for limited resources (Gire, 2019; Kornadt et al., 2022).

A bar graph shows the percentage of older adults in the population across several countries. It shows Qatar at 2%, Uganda at 3%, Ethiopia at 4%, Honduras at 5%, Brazil at 9%, Ireland at 14%, United States at 17%, Germany at 23%, and Japan at 29%.
Figure 16.3 The percentage of adults over age sixty-five years in the population varies drastically across countries, representing less than 5 percent in some and more than 20 percent in others. Countries with higher percentages of older adults in the population tend to have more negative cultural views about aging. (data source: Population Reference Bureau; attribution: Copyright Rice University, OpenStax, under CC BY 4.0 license)

Ageism and Stereotypes about Late Adulthood

Stereotypes and misconceptions about the aging process and older people are common, particularly when it is a stage of life we have not experienced ourselves. These views can be positive, such as associating older people with wisdom (Gire, 2019), but most tend to be negative, characterizing older adults as cognitively impaired, grumpy, depressed, isolated, in poor health, and unable to care for themselves (Hummert et al., 1994; Reissmann et al., 2021). Scientific research contradicts many of these biases.

A larger concern occurs when stereotypes translate to discrimination, which is called ageism when it is against someone solely due to their age. It is illegal in the workplace in the United States (under the Age Discrimination in Employment Act of 1967), yet research suggests it is still common, especially in the hiring process. Older applicants find it more difficult to gain employment despite their experience and qualifications (Dennis & Thomas, 2007; Neumark, 2020). Common stereotypes are that older workers underperform, resist change, are unable to learn new skills, and are distracted by family and health issues (Ilişanu & Andrei, 2018).

Older women are more likely to experience workplace ageism than older men (Walker & Zelin, 2021). This “gendered ageism” may be particularly true for certain positions or career fields, such as technology (Corbett, 2019), management positions, and physically demanding occupations (McGann et al., 2016). The combination of sexism and ageism in the workplace may lead to lower pay and a lack of promotion opportunities, producing gender differences in financial stability after retirement (Rochon et al., 2021).

The intersection of ageism and sexism also illuminates double standards in the wider culture. Popular entertainment, for instance, often pairs older men actors with much younger women. Data from the dating website OkCupid suggest that younger users, regardless of gender, want to find partners similar in age (Engel, 2014; Kenrick, 2018; Rudder, 2014). However, men users in their fifties tended to click on potential partners twenty-five years younger (Figure 16.4). Marriage data are consistent with this trend. Women marry similarly aged partners across the lifespan, but as men get older, the age gap between them and women widens (England & McClintock, 2009).

A bar graph shows preference of partner age on OK Cupid. It shows women in their 20s prefer men their age and men in their 20s prefer women 5 years younger. Women in their 30s prefer slightly younger men, while men in their 30s prefer women 15 years younger. Women in their 40s prefer men 5 years younger, while men in their 40s prefer women almost 25 years younger. Women at 50 prefer men about 4 years older while men at 50 prefer women 27 years younger.
Figure 16.4 Women using OkCupid expressed interest in similarly aged partners, while men looked for relatively younger partners as they got older. (data source: Rudder, Dataclysm: Who we are (when we think no one’s looking); attribution: Copyright Rice University, OpenStax, under CC BY 4.0 license)

While older men and older women are both underrepresented in the media, older men actors are more likely to play higher-status roles, such as successful businesspeople. Some older actors, such as Harrison Ford (who is over eighty years old) and Samuel L. Jackson (in his seventies), continue to get lead roles in action movies. Media portrayals of older women, on the other hand, constitute only about 25 percent of characters over age fifty years and tend to either make the actresses look younger or reinforce negative aging stereotypes, such as their being cognitively impaired (Geena Davis Institute on Gender in Media, 2020). Recent efforts have been more inclusive. For example, the Netflix show Grace and Frankie depicts the friendship of two older adult women.

Psychosocial Theory of Development: Integrity versus Despair

Cultural attitudes affect some views of late life, but other views may be more internally determined. The final stage in Erikson’s theory depicts the struggle with what he called integrity versus despair (Erikson & Erikson, 1998). In this stage, when older adults realize they’re nearing the end of their life, they undertake life reviews. A life review is someone’s reflection on and evaluation of the life they have lived, including their accomplishments, relationships, and decisions, and the way past events have contributed to their life’s meaning. According to Erikson, during this process, some individuals will experience despair, including regret over bad decisions or missed opportunities, while others achieve integrity by accepting their life in its entirety and with a positive outlook.

Stage theories have been criticized for oversimplifying the continuous nature of human development (Pelaez et al., 2008), and psychoanalytic theories have been critiqued for using unscientific approaches (Skinner, 1956). While these criticisms apply to Erikson’s theory, data-driven research has found support for Erikson's theory and its many components (Staudinger, 2001; Villar et al., 2021). For example, life reviews are being used therapeutically, especially in locations such as long-term care facilities that house older adults (Haber, 2006; Zhong et al., 2023). Research has found that for nursing home residents, engaging in a life review is associated with positive outcomes such as increasing aspects of integrity, decreasing aspects of despair, and improving quality of life (Haight et al., 2000; Zhong et al., 2023).

Erikson’s work has also influenced other developmental scholars. For example, Robert Peck expanded the integrity versus despair stage to include three additional tasks:

  • Differentiation is the process of adjusting your identity after retiring and finding meaning separate from work, such as by pursuing hobbies or volunteering.
  • Body transcendence is the act of coming to terms with the typical declines in physical ability and health. This includes finding value and meaning in areas of life that are not primarily dependent on physical abilities, such as engaging more in puzzles or games that are not physically difficult to perform.
  • Ego transcendence is the process of acknowledging and accepting that death is approaching. It includes reflecting on life and recognizing its meaning and value (Peck, 1956). This aspect of Peck’s theory is most consistent with Erikson’s idea of a life review.

Other research has explored the notion of a stage of development after integrity versus despair. Some of this research was based on the work of Joan Erikson, Erik Erikson’s spouse, who proposed that challenges faced after age eighty years required additional adjustment (Brown & Lowis, 2003; Erikson & Erikson, 1998). Tornstam (2011) referred to this stage as gerotranscendence, in which people in their eighties and nineties look beyond their own experience, consider their connections to earlier generations, and accept change as inevitable and natural. Evidence for gerotranscendence has been found in studies of Iranian, Japanese, Ethiopian, and British older adults, although it may be affected by cultural views of spirituality and individual characteristics such as tolerance for ambiguity (Brown & Lowis, 2003; Kida et al., 2024; Mekonnen et al., 2023; Raeesi Dehkordi et al., 2020).

Self and Social Development

People often interpret the word “self” as focusing on the individual, but the self is a very social topic, because people define and evaluate themselves relative to others (Adame, 2022; Sedikides et al., 2011). By comparing your characteristics and abilities to other people, you develop clear ideas about who you are and how you differ from others.

Some research focusing on the self in later life has examined how older adults define themselves. These definitions often include psychological and cognitive characteristics, interpersonal relationships, social roles, appearance, physical functioning, and attitudes (Freund & Smith, 1999; Lu, 2017; Pontinen et al., 2019). These domains may not be equally emphasized, however. Concerns about health and physical functioning often figure prominently (Pontinen et al., 2019), and a study of Israeli older adults noted a tendency to downplay aging and emphasize being active and young (Okun & Ayalon, 2023).

Older adults’ sense of self often includes both positive and negative views of their own aging, their changing appearance, their sense of aging well, their meaning in life, and their future hopes and fears (Hausknecht et al., 2020; Okun & Ayalon, 2023; Pontinen et al., 2019). Many older adults continue to define themselves in terms of their professional occupation despite likely being retired, and concerns about body image rank low on the list, suggesting that older adults are less preoccupied with their appearance and more connected to their younger and middle-aged identities than perhaps previously assumed. Two aspects of self—personality and emotions—are important parts of development at this life stage.

Personality in Late Adulthood

Research related to the Big Five traits (openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism) suggests they become very stable after age thirty years (Costa & McCrae, 1994), and this pattern has been found cross-culturally (Fung, 2013). Nonetheless, minor changes may occur (Allemand et al., 2008). Extraversion and conscientiousness slightly decline across adulthood, particularly after age sixty years, suggesting that retirement could reduce opportunities for social interaction and the need for the organized, careful behavior indicative of conscientiousness (Graham et al., 2020). Extraversion and conscientiousness are positively correlated with greater life satisfaction and better mental health in late adulthood, perhaps because they contribute to more and higher-quality relationships (Kida et al., 2024). Neuroticism decreases for most of adulthood but increases in later years, possibly due to anxiety about health problems and thoughts of mortality. Openness to new experiences appears to be stable across adulthood before decreasing in later life. Agreeableness did not demonstrate any change (Graham et al., 2020); this is worth noting because decreased agreeableness could indicate depression, cognitive decline, or other health problems.

Emotional Changes

One way to consider older adults’ emotional state is to examine research studying positive affect and negative affect. Positive affect is the extent to which someone experiences positive emotions such as happiness. Negative affect is the degree to which people feel negative emotions such as sadness or anger. Research suggests that as people age, they report experiencing more positive affect and less negative affect (Carstensen & DeLiema, 2018; Erbey et al., 2020; Mroczek & Kolarz, 1998) (Figure 16.5). This is thought to be one of the reasons depression decreases in older age.

Two line graphs show the negative and positive affect across the lifespan. It shows that the positive increase from 20 to 23.5 between the years 25 and 74, while the negative decreases from 10 to 8.5 between the years 25 and 74.
Figure 16.5 Positive affect increases throughout most of adulthood and older age, while there is a corresponding decline in negative affect. (attribution: Copyright Rice University, OpenStax, under CC BY 4.0 license)

The ability to control or regulate emotions can follow two paths when people age. One is to be as emotionally positive as possible, which may reflect acceptance of inevitable life changes and potential benefits of aging, such as wisdom, and is consistent with the themes of integrity and gerotranscendence (Kida et al., 2024; Mekonnen et al., 2023; Raeesi Dehkordi et al., 2020). Older adults tend to choose activities that result in positive emotional responses. For example, they avoid potentially distressing situations and seek out entertainment with more uplifting, optimistic themes (Bartsch, 2012; Erbey et al., 2020).

The other path lets us use our cognitive abilities to better understand our negative emotions and maintain objectivity when in a negative emotional state. Gisela Labouvie-Vief, for example, proposed a dynamic integration theory suggesting that emotion and cognition tend to increasingly merge throughout adulthood. At young ages, emotional responses tend to be simpler and more automatic, but they become increasingly complex with age because cognition and emotion are better integrated. Thus, people develop more tolerance for positive and negative emotional states and appreciate the complexity of their emotional responses (Labouvie-Vief, 2003; Labouvie-Vief et al., 2007). For example, say an older adult is in a negative emotional state after being diagnosed with a serious medical condition. They regulate this response and maintain objectivity by considering how they could improve their situation with medication and/or lifestyle changes. They may also consider people in poorer health than they are, allowing them to feel grateful for their current health. Thus, they use cognitive skills to better understand and tolerate negative emotions. This ability to consolidate cognition and emotion tends to decline with decreasing fluid intelligence in later years (Labouvie‐Vief, 2015).

References

Adame, A. L. (2022). Self-in-relation: Martin Buber and D. W. Winnicott in dialogue. The Humanistic Psychologist, 50(3), 376–388. https://doi.org/10.1037/hum0000203

Allemand, M., Zimprich, D., & Hendriks, A. A. J. (2008). Age differences in five personality domains across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 44(3), 758–770. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.758

Bartsch, A. (2012). As time goes by: What changes and what remains the same in entertainment experience over the life span? Journal of Communication, 62(4), 588–608. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01657.x

Brown, C., & Lowis, M. J. (2003). Psychosocial development in the elderly: An investigation into Erikson's ninth stage. Journal of Aging Studies, 17(4), 415–426. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0890-4065(03)00061-6

Carstensen, L. L., & DeLiema, M. (2018). The positivity effect: A negativity bias in youth fades with age. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 19, 7–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.07.009

Corbett, C. M. (2019). A young man’s game: Age and gender in technology jobs. In M. Choroszewicz & T. L. Adams (Eds.), Gender, age and inequality in the professions: Exploring the disordering, disruptive and chaotic properties of communication (pp. 213–243). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781351052467

Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1994). Set like plaster? Evidence for the stability of adult personality. In T. F. Heatherton & J. L Weinberger (Eds.), Can personality change? (pp. 21–40). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10143-002

Dehkordi, F. R., Eslami, A. A., Zamani Alavijeh, F., & Matlabi, H. (2020). Gerotranscendence and active aging: The lived experience. Journal of Religion, Spirituality & Aging, 33(3), 271–297. https://doi.org/10.1080/15528030.2020.1770662

Dennis, H. M., & Thomas, K. A. (2007). Ageism in the workplace. Generations, 31(1), 84–89. https://dspace2.creighton.edu/xmlui/handle/10504/58002

Engel, P. (2014, October 20). CHARTS: Guys like women in their early 20s regardless of how old they get. Business Insider. https://www.businessinsider.com/dataclysm-shows-men-are-attracted-to-women-in-their-20s-2014-10

England, P., & McClintock, E. A. (2009). The gendered double standard of aging in US marriage markets. Population and Development Review, 35(4), 797–816. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1728-4457.2009.00309.x

Erbey, M., Roebbig, J., Babayan, A., Kumral, D., Reinelt, J., Reiter, A. M. F., Schaare, L., Uhlig, M., Nierhaus, T., Van der Meer, E., Gaebler, M., & Villringer, A. (2020). Positivity in younger and in older age: Associations with future time perspective and socioemotional functioning. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 567133. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.567133

Erikson, E. H., & Erikson, J. M. (1998). The life cycle completed (extended version). W. W. Norton & Company.

Freund, A. M., & Smith, J. (1999). Content and function of the self-definition in old and very old age. The Journals of Gerontology Series B, 54B(1), 55–67. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/54b.1.p55

Fung, H. H. (2013). Aging in culture. The Gerontologist, 53(3), 369–377. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnt024

Geena Davis Institute on Gender in Media. (2020). Frail, frumpy and forgotten: A report on the movie roles of women of age. https://geenadavisinstitute.org/wp-content/uploads/2024/01/frail-frumpy-and-forgotten-report.pdf

Gire, J. T. (2019). Cultural variations in perceptions of aging. In K. D. Keith (Ed.), Cross‐cultural psychology: Contemporary themes and perspectives (pp. 110–129). John Wiley & Sons.

Graham, E. K., Weston, S. J., Gerstorf, D., Yoneda, T., Booth, T., Beam, C. R., Petkus, A. J., Drewelies, J., Hall, A. N., Bastarache, E. D., Estabrook, R., Katz, M. J., Turiano, N. A., Lindenberger, U., Smith, J., Wagner, G. G., Pedersen, N. L., Allemand, M., Spiro, A., III, . . . Mroczek, D. K. (2020). Trajectories of big five personality traits: A coordinated analysis of 16 longitudinal samples. European Journal of Personality, 34(3), 301–321. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2259

Haber, D. (2006). Life review: Implementation, theory, research, and therapy. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 63(2), 153–171. https://doi.org/10.2190/da9g-rhk5-n9jp-t6cc

Haight, B. K., Michel, Y., & Hendrix, S. (2000). The extended effects of the life review in nursing home residents. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 50(2), 151–168. https://doi.org/10.2190/qu66-e8uv-nymr-y99e

Hausknecht, S., Low, L.-F., O’Loughlin, K., McNab, J., & Clemson, L. (2020). Older adults’ self-perceptions of aging and 6being older: A scoping review. The Gerontologist, 60(7), e524–e534. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnz153

Hummert, M. L., Garstka, T. A., Shaner, J. L., & Strahm, S. (1994). Stereotypes of the elderly held by young, middle-aged, and elderly adults. Journal of Gerontology, 49(5), 240–249. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/49.5.p240

Ilişanu, G., & Andrei, V. (2018). Age stereotypes and ageism at the workplace – #ageisjustanumber. Journal of Comparative Research in Anthropology and Sociology, 9(2), 23–33. https://doaj.org/article/73b4ce9266694d64be7193103bd6f20e

Keldal, G., & Şeker, G. (2021). Marriage or career? Young adults’ priorities in their life plans. American Journal of Family Therapy, 50(5), 459–474. https://doi.org/10.1080/01926187.2021.1915213

Kenrick, D. T. (2018, February 13). Why are older hollywood actors paired with such young women? Psychology Today. https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/sex-murder-and-the-meaning-life/201802/why-are-older-hollywood-actors-paired-such-young-women

Kida, H., Niimura, H., Eguchi, Y., Suzuki, K., Shikimoto, R., Bun, S., Takayama, M., & Mimura, M. (2024). Relationship between life satisfaction and psychological characteristics among community-dwelling oldest-old: Focusing on Erikson's developmental stages and the big five personality traits. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 32(6), 724–735. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jagp.2023.12.018

Kornadt, A. E., de Paula Couto, C., & Rothermund, K. (2022). Views on aging—Current trends and future directions for cross-cultural research. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 6(2), 5. https://doi.org/10.9707/2307-0919.1176

Labouvie-Vief, G. (2003). Dynamic integration: Affect, cognition, and the self in adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(6), 201–206. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0963-7214.2003.01262.x

Labouvie‐Vief, G. (2015). Integrating emotions and cognition throughout the lifespan. In Springer Link. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-09822-7

Labouvie-Vief, G., Diehl, M., Jain, E., & Zhang, F. (2007). Six-year change in affect optimization and affect complexity across the adult life span: A further examination. Psychology and Aging, 22(4), 738–751. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.22.4.738

Löckenhoff, C. E., De Fruyt, F., Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R., De Bolle, M., Costa, P. T., Jr., Aguilar-Vafaie, M. E., Ahn, C.-k., Ahn, H.-n., Alcalay, L., Allik, J., Avdeyeva, T. V., Barbaranelli, C., Benet-Martínez, V., Blatný, M., Bratko, D., Cain, T. R., Crawford, J. T., Lima, M. P., . . . Yik, M. (2009). Perceptions of aging across 26 cultures and their culture-level associates. Psychology and Aging, 24(4), 941–954. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016901

Lu, L. (2017). Creating well-being among older people: An Eastern perspective. In C. L. Cooper & J. C. Quick (Eds.), The handbook of stress and health: A guide to research and practice (1st ed., pp. 388–399). John Wiley & Sons. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118993811.ch23

McGann, M., Ong, R., Bowman, D., Duncan, A., Kimberley, H., & Biggs, S. (2016). Gendered ageism in Australia: Changing perceptions of age discrimination among older men and women. Economic Papers: A Journal of Applied Economics and Policy, 35(4), 375–388. https://doi.org/10.1111/1759-3441.12155

Mekonnen, H. S., Lindgren, H., Geda, B., Azale, T., & Erlandsson, K. (2023). Being an elderly person living in metropolitan cities of northwestern Ethiopia: A descriptive phenomenological study. Clinical Interventions in Aging, 18, 205–218. https://doi.org/10.2147/CIA.S389305

Mroczek, D. K., & Kolarz, C. M. (1998). The effect of age on positive and negative affect: A developmental perspective on happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(5), 1333–1349. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.5.1333

Neumark, D. (2020). Age discrimination in hiring: Evidence from age-blind vs. non-age-blind hiring procedures. National Bureau of Economic Research. https://doi.org/10.3386/w26623

Okun, S., & Ayalon, L. (2023). The paradox of subjective age: Age(ing) in the self-presentation of older adults. International Psychogeriatrics, 35(10), 566–575. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610222000667

Peck, R. N. (1956). Psychological developments in the second half of life. In J. E. Anderson (Ed.), Psychological aspects of aging (pp. 42–53). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10032-005

Pelaez, M., Gewirtz, J. L., & Wong, S. E. (2008). A critique of stage theories of human development: A pragmatic approach in social work. In B. A. Thyer, K. M. Sowers, & C. N. Dulmus (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of social work and social welfare, vol. 2: Human behavior in the social environment (pp. 503–518). John Wiley & Sons.

Pontinen, H. M., Medvene, L. J., & Gerstenkorn, J. (2019). Possible selves of older adults entering a life plan community. Educational Gerontology, 45(3), 201–213. https://doi.org/10.1080/03601277.2019.1601400

Population Reference Bureau. (2020). Countries with the oldest populations in the world. https://www.prb.org/resources/countries-with-the-oldest-populations-in-the-world/

Reissmann, M., Geithner, L., Storms, A., & Woopen, C. (2021). Stereotypes about very old people and perceived societal appreciation in very old age. Zeitschrift für Gerontologie und Geriatrie, 54(S2), 93–100. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00391-021-01971-y

Rochon, P. A., Kalia, S., & Higgs, P. (2021). Gendered ageism: Addressing discrimination based on age and sex. The Lancet, 398(10301), 648–649. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(21)01636-6

Rudder, C. (2014). Dataclysm: Love, sex, race, and identity—What our online lives tell us about our offline selves. Crown.

Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & O'Mara, E. M. (2011). Individual self, relational self, collective self: Hierarchical ordering of the tripartite self. Psychological Studies, 56, 98–107. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-011-0059-0

Skinner, B. F. (1956). Critique of psychoanalytic concepts and theories. In H. Feigl & M.

Scriven (Eds.), Foundations of science and the concepts of psychology and psychoanalysis, 1, 77–-87. Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science. University of Minnesota Press. https://conservancy.umn.edu/items/1357be58-517f-4258-b539-379100624bcf

Staudinger, U. M. (2001). Life reflection: A social-cognitive analysis of life review. Review of General Psychology, 5(2), 148–160. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.5.2.148

Strout, K., Ahmed, F., Sporer, K., Howard, E. P., Sassatelli, E., & Mcfadden, K. (2018). What are older adults wellness priorities? A qualitative analysis of priorities within multiple domains of wellness. Healthy Aging Research, 7(2), e21. https://doi.org/10.1097/hxr.0000000000000021

Tornstam, L. (2011). Maturing into gerotranscendence. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 43(2), 166–180. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2014-01047-007

Vauclair, C.-M., Hanke, K., Huang, L.-L., & Abrams, D. (2016). Are Asian cultures really less ageist than Western ones? It depends on the questions asked. International Journal of Psychology, 52(2), 136–144. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.12292

Villar, F., Serrat, R., & Pratt, M. W. (2021). Older age as a time to contribute: A scoping review of generativity in later life. Ageing & Society, 43(8), 1860–1881. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0144686x21001379

Walker, R. V., & Zelin, A. I. (2021). “You’re too young/old for this”: The intersection of ageism and sexism in the workplace. In E. Cole & L. Hollis-Sawyer (Eds.), Older women who work: Resilience, choice, and change (pp. 161–187). American Psychological Association eBooks. https://doi.org/10.1037/0000212-010

Wettstein, M., Park, R., Kornadt, A. E., Wurm, S., Ram, N., & Gerstorf, D. (2024). Postponing old age: Evidence for historical change toward a later perceived onset of old age. Psychology and Aging, 39(5), 526–541. APA PsycNet. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000812

Zhong, Q., Chen, C., & Chen, S. (2023). Effectiveness on quality of life and life satisfaction for older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis of life review and reminiscence therapy across settings. Behavioral Sciences, 13(10), 830. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13100830

Footnotes

  • 1This study (Lockenhoff et al., 2009) uses the terms “Asian” and “Western.” This study (Wettstein et al., 2024) used German research participants.
Citation/Attribution

This book may not be used in the training of large language models or otherwise be ingested into large language models or generative AI offerings without OpenStax's permission.

Want to cite, share, or modify this book? This book uses the Creative Commons Attribution License and you must attribute OpenStax.

Attribution information
  • If you are redistributing all or part of this book in a print format, then you must include on every physical page the following attribution:
    Access for free at https://openstax.org/books/lifespan-development/pages/1-what-does-psychology-say
  • If you are redistributing all or part of this book in a digital format, then you must include on every digital page view the following attribution:
    Access for free at https://openstax.org/books/lifespan-development/pages/1-what-does-psychology-say
Citation information

© Oct 2, 2024 OpenStax. Textbook content produced by OpenStax is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License . The OpenStax name, OpenStax logo, OpenStax book covers, OpenStax CNX name, and OpenStax CNX logo are not subject to the Creative Commons license and may not be reproduced without the prior and express written consent of Rice University.