Skip to ContentGo to accessibility pageKeyboard shortcuts menu
OpenStax Logo
Lifespan Development

1.2 Themes of Development

Lifespan Development1.2 Themes of Development

Learning Objectives

By the end of this section, you will be able to:

  • Explain the difference between continuous and discontinuous development
  • Describe nature and nurture as sources for developmental change
  • Explain the importance of the timing of opportunities for development

Maya, who is in her late forties, never understood the draw of social media. She put off creating a Facebook account until she found herself missing out on information about her niece’s school shared via Facebook groups. Now she finds herself reconnecting with old friends and acquaintances from high school and neighborhoods where she’s lived, almost like a virtual reunion. She is fascinated by how people have both changed and stayed the same. Maya’s college girlfriend, who swore that she never wanted children, is now a mother of four. But she held to her goal of joining—and eventually running—her family’s business. Maya’s timid high school classmate took a leap and now lives an adventurous life as a travel writer. But the writer part doesn’t surprise Maya at all. She always turned to this friend for support on school papers.

To a developmental psychologist, such a scenario is endlessly fascinating. People often have a natural inclination to wonder about how people from our past turned out. The field of lifespan development has established and advanced several themes and questions to focus this curiosity and exploration. These themes look at the form of, sources for, and timing of developmental experiences and achievements.

Continuous versus Discontinuous Development

The first theme that helps organize the way we think about the nature of development over time is the shape of developmental change (Figure 1.5). Theories about this developmental change are often called stage theories: a stage theory attempts to explain why we may observe brief periods of rapid development followed by longer stretches of stability. Stage theories give us an idea about the nature of change as well. Relatively abrupt change is likely to be more apparent and cause us to wonder about its source or mechanism.

When we observe a gradual day-by-day or week-over-week progression of change, we see continuous development. Many topics in the field of lifespan development show a continuous pattern of development. For example, vocabulary growth across the toddler and early childhood years shows a dramatic but smooth progression of growth in the number of words a child produces and comprehends (e.g., Pan et al., 2005).

However, early psychological theorists, such as Jean Piaget and Erik Erikson (whose work you’ll learn about throughout this course), noted that certain cognitive and personality characteristics seem to develop in intermittent fits and spurts—or stages. For a period of time, the developing individual has one set of capabilities, such as a toddler who communicates in two-word utterances, and then, rather abruptly, a reorganization takes place, and the individual is transformed. Seemingly overnight, that toddler begins speaking in complete sentences. This is discontinuous development—a change in developmental kind, form, or degree that does not directly follow from what came before. Some developmental progressions may appear to be discontinuous, such as the sudden changes of puberty. However, when researchers consider the biological changes that prepare a body for the growth spurts of puberty, such as hormone changes, we begin to see that continuous development patterns are also present.

Illustration showing Continuous Development as an incline from Infancy (lower end) through Adulthood and Discontinuous Development as various colored steps from Infancy (shorter steps) to Adulthood (taller steps).
Figure 1.5 In contrasting theories, development can be viewed as continuous or discontinuous. Continuous development is conceptualized as a gradual, ongoing process, whereas discontinuous development is marked by distinct, stepped stages. (attribution: Copyright Rice University, OpenStax, under CC BY 4.0 license)

Whether a characteristic appears to follow a continuous or a discontinuous pattern, an underlying assumption in developmental psychology is the idea that development is progressive and proceeds from basic forms of a characteristic in early life to more advanced forms later. That is, development moves forward. From this perspective, moving backward to an early stage or lower capability, a process termed regression, is a sign that something may be wrong. In order to promote early child development, major organizations like the United Nations International Children’s Emergency Fund (UNICEF) and the World Health Organization (WHO) have made it a priority to educate caregivers about the importance of psychosocial development (Lucas et al. 2018). For example, poor nutrition can lead to delayed physical growth and poorer long-term health. However, interventions that provide nutrition supplementation in early childhood can help children catch up on physical growth and improve health outcomes (Lucas et al., 2018).

Sources of Developmental Change

We now turn to the theme for which developmental psychology is best known: the relationship between nature and nurture. You may also hear this referred to as the nature versus nurture debate (Angoff, 1988). Suppose we observe a developmental change (such as a growth spurt in height) or a state of stability in an individual (such as having a consistent outgoing personality), and their genetic blueprint appears to be the reason for it. In that case, we argue that nature or biological forces are directing the changes or working to keep the characteristics consistent. On the other hand, if the impetus for change seems to come from outside the individual, whether it be a physical or a social aspect of the environment (such as engaging in a new hobby or growing up in a certain family dynamic), then we argue that nurture (the environment) is the reason.

For most physical and psychological characteristics, the source for developmental change or stability is rarely nature or nurture alone. Instead, developmental psychologists focus on the complex and fascinating ways that nature and nurture combine and work together to direct change and stability across the lifespan (Figure 1.6).

Adult and child with stuffed animal walk through forest holding hands.
Figure 1.6 A father and son, side by side, mirror the growth of the trees they walk through. Development and change are influenced by both biological and environmental factors. (credit: “Man in Black Jacket Beside Boy in Pink Jacket Holding Plush Toy during Daytime” by Quaries Official/Quaries.com, CC BY 2.0)

Heritability

Until a few decades ago, the field of psychology often explored the question of what caused developmental change by asking whether it was nature or nurture. Due to advances in the discipline’s methodologies, findings, and theories, we now think in terms of nature and nurture, recognizing that most development has both genetic (nature) and environmental (nurture) influences. In fact, nearly every psychological characteristic is composed of a combination of biological genetic components and environmental influences. The amount of influence from genetics and environment varies depending on the characteristic. Today, psychologists work to discover exactly how much each source contributes. The extent to which the genetic component explains difference is called a heritability estimate. A good example of how nature and nurture both contribute is the timing of the onset of puberty. It is determined via the combined influence of nature and nurture, given that both biological determinants (nature) like genes, and environmental elements (nurture) like nutrition, can trigger its emergence.

Closely related to our thinking about the relative contributions of nature and nurture is the concept of reaction range (Figure 1.7). This is the idea that our genes likely set upper and lower levels for particular traits, behaviors, and abilities, as well as how sensitive these are to environmental forces. One example to consider is general intelligence (Sternberg, 2012). While scientists have yet to fully identify the specific genes responsible for intelligence, we know it’s not solely determined by genetics: environmental factors are of equal importance. However, it’s difficult to directly measure the impact of these environmental influences because researchers cannot ethically manipulate a child’s upbringing to observe the effects on their intelligence. Although the application of reaction range to intelligence remains largely theoretical, it serves as a useful way to conceptualize the complex interplay of genetics and environment in human development. It reminds us that both nature and nurture contribute to who we become.

Alt text
Figure 1.7 Although how the contributing factors combine in complex ways not fully understood, researchers agree that both genes and environment play roles in the development of intelligence. (attribution: Copyright Rice University, OpenStax, under CC BY 4.0 license)

Gene-Environment Correlations

Another way developmental psychologists try to discern the relative contributions of genetic and environmental influences is gene-environment correlation. Gene-environment correlation describes the complex interplay between our genes and the environments we experience (Price & Jaffee, 2008), and encompasses three main types: passive, evocative, and active.

  • The passive type occurs when a child inherits genes from their parents that influence the environment in which they are raised. For example, parents who enjoy reading are more likely to read to their children regularly and have many books in the house.
  • The evocative type explains how our genetically influenced behaviors can evoke reactions in others, shaping our environment in turn. For example, parents are more likely to take their child to the library if they notice the child enjoys reading.
  • The active type refers to how our genetic tendencies guide us to select specific environments. For example, a child joins a book reading club at school because they have a passion for reading.

Studies of twins and adopted children have shown that our genes play a role in shaping the environments we experience, highlighting that what seems like environmental influence may actually be partly due to our genetic makeup.

Epigenetics

A breakthrough concept over the past few decades called epigenetics also explores the complexity of the nature and nurture question, as well as the level of sophistication in our current thinking about how best to answer it. The process of epigenetics explains how an individual’s behaviors and environment can cause changes that affect the way their genes work. (Smeeth et al., 2021). Biologists, neuroscientists, and psychologists alike now recognize that environmental influences can modify an individual’s genetic expression. There is evidence that stressful environments can trigger a chemical tagging of someone’s DNA. For example, trauma experienced in childhood (like growing up in a highly stressful home environment, violence-ridden neighborhood, or a war zone) could make someone sensitized to and highly reactive to signs of conflict later in life (Gladish et al., 2022; Ramo-Fernández et al., 2015). More specifically, the genes responsible for building the brain systems that regulate our stress level could be altered. The nature and nurture question is as fascinating to explore as it is complex to answer, and it underlies just about every topic of inquiry studied by lifespan psychologists.

Windows of Opportunity for Development

In addition to studying what changes are expected and how they occur, developmental psychologists ask when we should expect certain skills to develop, and whether there are windows of opportunity for these that affect developmental outcomes. Is it possible to speed up development if we introduce an experience at just the right time? Is it possible to hinder or even prevent the development of a particular ability or characteristic altogether, such as speech? What are the impacts of highly enriching environments? What are the impacts of being deprived of certain experiences, such as human contact?

Scientists have learned that across nearly all psychological characteristics, humans are highly adaptable. They observe normative developmental outcomes, meaning those that are typical or expected, across a wide range of environmental conditions. Certainly, there are optimal environments for these developmental outcomes, but good outcomes occur even in suboptimal circumstances. This is central to resilience, an individual’s capacity for and “process of adapting well in the face of adversity, trauma, tragedy, threats or even significant sources of stress” (American Psychological Association [APA], 2014). It takes extreme deprivation to severely restrict a developing human’s potential, as well as such deprivation occurring at specific developmental times. In other words, resilience is common and lifespan development principles can be applied to increase the likelihood of resilience.

A critical period is the developmental age range in which certain experiences are required for a psychological or physical ability to develop (Colombo et al., 2019). For example, it appears that exposure to human speech is necessary in the early years of life for typical language development. Consider the 1970s case of Genie, a child who was severely neglected and isolated to the point that she was rarely spoken to. Upon her rescue at age thirteen years, Genie faced the monumental task of learning a language (a primary task of infants and toddlers) with an adolescent (post-pubescent) brain (Jones, 1995). Genie became the subject of intense study and remediation efforts by doctors, speech pathologists, and psychologists (Fromkin et al., 1974). Despite her extreme early deprivation, she made many improvements in language comprehension and speech production. However, her development of language differed markedly from what is normative. For example, language production and comprehension happen in the left hemisphere of the brain for the vast majority of humans, but Genie showed processing of language in her right cerebral hemisphere.

A sensitive period describes an age range during which the development of a characteristic is particularly amenable to inputs and influences from the environment (Colombo et al., 2019). For a critical period, certain experiences are necessary for normal development, whereas having certain experiences during a sensitive period makes an advantageous situation for ideal development (Pascalis, 2020). For example, a sports coach would want to know about normative periods for growth spurts in males and females, especially the typically wide variation around puberty. Not only could this awareness of timing help with training young athletes, but it could inform injury prevention practices. A faith-based youth leader working closely with a group of teenagers would want to understand that the adolescent period is a time of identity exploration and desire to fit in with peers, and plan activities accordingly. A social worker employed by an assisted living facility knows that residents might be concerned with life review and may develop a scrapbooking activity to support the process of reflection. Physical development, identity exploration, and reflections on past life can occur across the entire lifespan. What’s notable across these three examples is that each takes on more importance at a different stage in life, when the opportunities for impact on development are greatest. That is the concept of the sensitive period.

Variability and Diversity of Development

In this course, you’ll learn about numerous theories, topics, and research findings based on the developmental experiences of many thousands of people. It is sometimes easy to confuse or conflate the broad results of studies with individual life histories. Scientific ideas about development represent statistical averages for developmental outcomes across many individuals. But every individual is unique and has their own life history—one that may show varying degrees of similarity to and deviation from the findings presented as normative in this discipline.

For example, when you learn that two-word utterances typically appear around eighteen months, a reader might recall, “Well, my child did this when they were twelve months old.” The fact that this individual’s development doesn’t perfectly fit the finding doesn’t invalidate the overall research. What’s more important is the pattern or sequence from one-word utterances to two, and that this development occurs within a typical timeframe, the midpoint of thousands of examples with a range of variations on either side. So, while lifespan psychology attempts to uncover what is typical for all humans, psychologists recognize and allow for the inherent variability of a single individual’s development.

Variability in development can also occur on a cultural level. For example, research compared Dutch and Israeli parents and found cultural differences in how they promote their babies’ motor development (Oudgenoeg-Paz et al., 2020). Israeli parents encouraged more tummy time, believing it helps babies develop faster. Dutch parents were not as likely to encourage their infants spend time laying on their tummies. Interestingly, the Israeli babies who got more tummy time and stimulation from their parents showed better crawling skills than the Dutch babies. This study highlights that different cultures have different approaches to parenting, and these choices can influence how babies develop.

References

American Psychological Association. (2024). Resilience. https://www.apa.org/topics/resilience

Angoff, W. H. (1988). The nature-nurture debate, aptitudes, and group differences. American Psychologist, 43(9), 713–720. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.43.9.713

Colombo, J. Gustafson, K. M., & Carlson, S. E. (2019). Critical and sensitive periods in development and nutrition [Supplemental material]. Annals of Nutrition and Metabolism, 75(1), 34–42. https://doi.org/10.1159/000508053

Fromkin, V., Krashen, S., Curtiss, S., Rigler, D., & Rigler, M. (1974). The development of language in Genie: A case of language acquisition beyond the “critical period.” Brain and Language, 1(1), 81–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(74)90027-3

Gladish, N., Merrill, S. M., & Kobor, M. S. (2022). Childhood trauma and epigenetics: State of the science and future. Current Environmental Health Reports, 9(4), 661–672. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40572-022-00381-5

Jaffe, A. E., Straub, R.E., Shin, J. H., Tao, R., Gao, Y., Collado-Torres, L., Kam-Thong, T., Xi, H. S., Quan, J., Chen, Q., Colantuoni, C., Ulrich, W. S., Maher, B. J., Deep-Soboslay, A., The BrainSeq Consortium, Cross, A. J., Brandon, N. J., Leek, J. T., Hyde, T. M., Kleinman, J. E., & Weinberger, D. R. (2018). Developmental and genetic regulation of the human cortex transcriptome illuminate schizophrenia pathogenesis. Nature Neuroscience, 21, 1117–1125. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0197-y

Jones, P.E., (1995). Contradictions and unanswered questions in the Genie case: A fresh look at the linguistic evidence. Language & Communication, 15(3), 261–280. https://doi.org/10.1016/0271-5309(95)00007-D

Lucas, J. E., Richter, L. M., & Daelmans, B. (2018). Care for Child Development: an intervention in support of responsive caregiving and early child development. Child: Care, health and development, 44(1), 41–49. https://doi.org/10.1111/cch.12544

Oudgenoeg-Paz, O., Atun-Einy, O., & van Schaik, S. D. M. (2020). Two cultural models on infant motor development: Middle class parents in Israel and the Netherlands. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00119

Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., & Snow, C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development, 76(4), 763–782. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00498-i1

Pascalis, O., Fort, M., & Quinn, P. C. (2020). Development of face processing: Are there critical or sensitive periods? Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 36, 7–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.05.005

Price, T. S., & Jaffee, S. R. (2008). Effects of the family environment: gene-environment interaction and passive gene-environment correlation. Developmental Psychology, 44(2), 305. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.305

Ramo-Fernández, L., Schneider, A., Wilker, S., & Kolassa, I. T. (2015). Epigenetic alterations associated with war trauma and childhood maltreatment. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 33(5), 701–721. https://doi.org/10.1002/bsl.2200

Smeeth, D., Beck, S., Karam, E. G., & Pluess, M. (2021). The role of epigenetics in psychological resilience. Lancet Psychiatry, 8(7), 620–629. https://doi.org/10.1016/s2215-0366(20)30515-0

Sternberg, R. (2012). Intelligence. WIREs Cognitive Science, 3(4), 501–511. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1193

Citation/Attribution

This book may not be used in the training of large language models or otherwise be ingested into large language models or generative AI offerings without OpenStax's permission.

Want to cite, share, or modify this book? This book uses the Creative Commons Attribution License and you must attribute OpenStax.

Attribution information
  • If you are redistributing all or part of this book in a print format, then you must include on every physical page the following attribution:
    Access for free at https://openstax.org/books/lifespan-development/pages/1-what-does-psychology-say
  • If you are redistributing all or part of this book in a digital format, then you must include on every digital page view the following attribution:
    Access for free at https://openstax.org/books/lifespan-development/pages/1-what-does-psychology-say
Citation information

© Oct 2, 2024 OpenStax. Textbook content produced by OpenStax is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution License . The OpenStax name, OpenStax logo, OpenStax book covers, OpenStax CNX name, and OpenStax CNX logo are not subject to the Creative Commons license and may not be reproduced without the prior and express written consent of Rice University.